1. The choice and interpretation of subjects in anaphoric contexts
The choice and interpretation of subjects in anaphoric contexts (i.e. when they are used to resume a previously introduced discourse referent) is affected by a number of factors which the relevant literature has extensively addressed. An influential model is Ariel’s (1990; 2001) Accessibility Theory which assumes that choice/interpretation of an anaphoric referring expression is influenced by the prominence (accessibility) of the antecedent: the more prominent the antecedent, the less explicit the anaphoric device will be; the less explicit the anaphoric device, the more prominent the antecedent will be. Prominence of an antecedent is in turn a function of structural and discourse factors such as its subject/object status (with subjects more prominent than objects), its topicality (with topics more prominent than non-topics) and the presence/absence of competing antecedents. Accessibility Theory also assumes that whenever two forms are attested in a given language, they will occur in the same hierarchical order cross-linguistically.
As is well known languages differ with respect to the possibility of allowing sentences with a null subject. This possibility, i.e. the positive setting of the relevant parameter (Taraldsen 1980; Rizzi 1982) has a number of related subtle syntactic properties, such as the possibility of having post-verbal subjects, which are not, however, equally employed in all null subject languages (Gilligan 1987; Belletti 2001, 2004; Roussou & Tsimpli 2006; Ordoñez, 2007 a. o.). When we deal with anaphora resolution, we have thus a main divide, with non-null subject languages on one side (where only overt subjects – lexical or pronominal – can be used to retrieve a previously introduced discourse referent) and null subject languages on the other (which avail themselves of an extra device, i.e. pro, the null pronominal subject). This difference in the inventory of anaphoric devices is expected to have some impact on their antecedent retrieval properties.
An overt subject pronoun, for instance, is preferably interpreted as co-referent with a subject antecedent in a non-null subject language as English (Arnold et al. 2000; Arnold 2010), while in a null subject language as Italian, as we shall see, it is preferably interpreted in co-reference with a non-subject antecedent.12
It is also possible, however, that the division of labor among the different referring expression is not the same within the languages of each group, as we have seen above for the use of the post-verbal subject position available in null subject languages: a possibility made available by the grammar of some languages may be put to a different use in different languages sharing the same property.
Research on null subject languages has mainly focused on the null/overt pronoun division of labor, which has often been studied in fully ambiguous contexts, where a null or an overt pronoun and two possible antecedents for the pronoun are placed in bi-clausal sentences, as in (1) from Italian:
- (1)
- Quando
- when
- Mario
- Mario
- ha
- has
- incontrato
- met
- Gianni,
- Gianni
- pro/lui
- pro/he
- ha
- has
- sorriso
- smiled
- ‘When Mario met Gianni, he smiled’
In his seminal work, Calabrese (1986) proposed that in Italian, pro picks up an antecedent that is the subject of primary predication (or thema) of the preceding clause (Mario in (1)), while the overt pronoun picks up the ‘unexpected’ object antecedent (Gianni in (1)).
The characterization of the preferred antecedent of pro proposed by Calabrese (1986) ties together two of its peculiar properties that frequently co-occur (Rizzi 2005, 2018): the structural property of being a subject, and the d-linked (discourse linked) property of being a topic. As for the first property, Carminati (2002) has proposed the Position of Antecedent Hypothesis, providing experimental evidence confirming that pro in Italian is preferably interpreted as co-referent with an antecedent in Spec, IP, while an overt pronoun is preferably interpreted as co-referent with an object antecedent. The preferred interpretation of pro, however, seems to be weakened in certain structural environments, as also revealed by some experimental evidence (Carminati 2002, Sorace & Filiaci 2006), while it emerges particularly, as noted by Rizzi (2018), with preposed adverbials, such as (1), and in discourse sequences.3
It is also unclear whether the same interpretive preferences can be extended to other null subject languages. Filiaci (2010) and Filiaci, Sorace & Carreiras (2014), for instance, noted some differences between Italian and Spanish. In their self-paced reading study, Filiaci, Sorace & Carreiras (2014) found that while in Italian pro prefers an antecedent in subject position and the overt pronoun prefers a non-subject antecedent, the overt pronoun in Spanish has not a clear object bias. Chamorro, Sorace & Sturt (2016) and Chamorro (2018), instead, provided evidence suggesting that pro in Spanish does not have a clear subject bias. The clausal environments used by Chamorro (2018), however, were the same in which Sorace & Filiaci (2006) had found a weaker subject bias for pro in Italian, so it is possible, as Chamorro (2018: 13) suggests, that the unclear subject preference of pro in Spanish is akin to that found in Italian.4 The evidence concerning the comparison of Spanish and Italian, hence, is not conclusive and the issue of micro-variation in the division of labor among referring expressions in null subject languages certainly deserves further investigation, expanding as well the inquiry to other null subject languages.
Giannakou & Sitaridou (2020), examined native speakers of Greek and native speakers of Chilean Spanish in production and comprehension. They found a clear difference between Greek and Chilean Spanish in the use and interpretation of overt subject pronouns. In production, although the use of overt subject pronouns was low in both groups, frequency was significantly lower in Greek than in Spanish. In comprehension, while Greek speakers consistently linked the overt pronoun to the object, in Spanish there was no strong bias towards any of the antecedents. The authors hypothesize that the difference may be due to the fact that the Greek third person overt subject pronoun has the same form of the demonstrative, (aftós, in the masculine singular nominative form). To this end, they conceived a follow-up study in which Chilean Spanish speakers had to interpret the demonstrative (este in the masculine singular form) instead of the personal pronoun (él in the masculine singular form) of the previous study. Results confirm that the interpretation of este is akin to that of aftos, i.e. it has a comparable object bias.
Furthermore, Giannakou & Sitaridou (2020) found that in both Spanish and Greek null subjects were allowed in topic shift contexts in production, and did not exhibit a clear subject bias in comprehension: this could suggest a difference concerning null subjects with Spanish and Greek on one side and Italian on the other, where, according to Calabrese (1986) and Carminati (2002) null subjects have a strong topic/subject bias.5
Some of the studies in this collection address the issue of micro-variation in the division of labor between null and overt subject pronouns in null subject languages, but before examining them we would like to highlight another dimension of variation that the relevant literature has addressed: bilingual speakers (in particular late bilinguals, i.e. following Sorace 2016, speakers who have started to acquire a second language after puberty) of a null subject language appear to over-use overt pronouns with respect to monolingual speakers, and to interpret them as co-referent with a subject antecedent to a greater extent than monolinguals. Many studies have confirmed this finding in bilingual speakers of a non-null and a null subject language (Tsimpli et al. 2004; Sorace & Filiaci 2006; Belletti, Bennati & Sorace, 2007; Serratrice, Sorace & Paoli 2004 a. o.), identifying cross-linguistic influence as its source, in a specific direction: the non-null subject language seems to influence what bilinguals do in their null subject language, regardless of the fact that the non-null subject language is the first or the second language of these speakers.
Interestingly, however, some studies revealed an over-use of overt subject pronouns also in bilingual speakers of two null subject languages (Sorace et al. 2009; Margaza & Bel 2006; Lozano 2006, among many others). The question then is whether subtle differences among the two null subject languages spoken by the bilinguals play a role (and hence cross-linguistic influence is again involved) or the over-use of overt pronouns by bilinguals is a peculiarity of these speakers. This in turn raises the question of why it should be so, and whether bilinguals are simply prone to be over-explicit (Hendriks 2003 a. o.), or have processing difficulties which emerge in anaphora resolution. Anaphora resolution is indeed a complex domain, particularly vulnerable in bilingual acquisition (possibly because it is at the syntax-discourse interface, as proposed by the Interface Hypothesis, see Sorace & Filiaci 2006, Tsimpli & Sorace 2006, Sorace 2011) and acquired late in first language acquisition (Sorace et al. 2009).
These considerations in turn prompted a line of research aimed at inquiring on possible cognitive factors influencing anaphora resolution, as well as its acquisition, in different populations of speakers.
Age-related differences in particular (in connection perhaps to working memory capacities, van Rij et al. 2013) appear to be relevant: children display preferences in anaphora resolution which partly differ from those of adults (Papadopoulou et al. 2015; Hendriks et al. 2014); young and elderly adults also differ, particularly in production (Hendriks et al. 2014).
2. The present collection
The present collection contains seven contributions that address different aspects contributing to variation in subject realization and interpretation in monolingual and bilingual speakers, ranging from interlingual differences and syntactic constraints (word order, intervention), to prosody, discourse and cognitive factors. As far as the languages investigated are concerned, three null subject languages are considered (Italian, Greek and Spanish) either individually or in mutual comparison (Italian/Greek; Greek/Spanish), or in comparison with a non-null subject language (Spanish/English; Italian/Swedish). Most contributions focus on adult L1 speakers, often compared to adult L2 speakers. Children as well as ageing adults are also investigated in some of the studies. Various experimental techniques are employed to test interpretation and/or production, ranging from semi-spontaneous and elicited production to multiple-choice tasks and eye-tracking measures.
2.1 The role of prosody in overt pronoun resolution in a null subject language and in a non-null subject language: A production study
Chiara Gargiulo, Mechtild Tronnier & Petra Bernardini investigate how prosodic cues influence overt pronouns interpretation in Italian and Swedish. A production task and a control interpretation task were administered to 28 adult native speakers of Italian and 28 adult native speakers of Swedish. While in Italian, as we have seen, overt pronouns are preferably associated to object antecedents, in a non-null subject language like Swedish overt pronouns are assumed to be equally biased towards a subject or an object antecedent. In the production task, participants had to read out loud globally ambiguous bi-clausal sentences containing an overt pronoun and two possible antecedents (as in (1) above). Each sentence was preceded by a context sentence which made clear which was the intended antecedent (subject or object) for the pronoun. Results show that both Swedish and Italian participants use a longer inter-clausal pause and higher prominence (with prominence defined in terms of length, F0 and intensity) to signal the non-preferred interpretation of the overt pronoun. Variation lies in the interpretation preferences for the overt pronoun, which, as clearly revealed by the data of the interpretation task, display an object bias in Italian and a subject bias in Swedish.
2.2 The processing of subject pronouns in highly proficient L2 speakers of English
Carla Contemori & Paola Dussias deal with pronominal resolution in L2 speakers of a non-null subject language (English) whose L1 is Mexican Spanish. Previous studies on the acquisition of anaphora resolution in a non-null subject second language are quite rare, and have shown mixed results: while off-line tasks showed a native-like performance (Contemori et al. 2019), some studies based on on-line measures (Roberts et al. 2008) revealed a processing penalty experienced by L2ers when compared to native speakers, while other studies did not (Cunnings et al. 2017). The 24 L2 speakers examined by Contemori & Dussias – in comparison to a control group of 25 monolingual English speakers – are both late bilinguals and speakers who have been exposed to the L2 since early childhood having achieved a near-native competence. The authors use eye-tracking measures during listening (visual world paradigm) to explore whether gender information and the first-mention bias have similar rapid on-line effects during pronoun interpretation in the two groups of speakers. Their results did not show a delayed processing by L2ers when encountering the pronoun, but only an attenuated use of the gender information during pronoun resolution. When the complexity of the discourse is relatively low, as in the experimental materials of this study (as well as of Cunnings et al.’s), pronoun resolution by L2ers may not be associated with processing cost, even when their two languages have different sets of referring expressions and different interpretation biases, as English and Spanish.6
2.3 Null/overt subject alternations in L2 Spanish and L2 Greek
Panagiota Margaza & Anna Gavarró examine the distribution of null and overt subject pronouns in intermediate and advanced adult L2 speakers of Spanish (with L1 Greek) and of Greek (with L1 Spanish). Two control groups of L1 speakers of Spanish and L1 speakers of Greek were also involved, for a total of 180 participants (30 per group). The data were collected through two versions of a multiple-choice task (in fact, a two options choice, which for the experimental items was overt vs. null pronoun), one in Spanish and one in Greek. A quite unexpected result – with respect to the previous literature – is that Greek native speakers allowed a significantly higher rate of overt subjects than Spanish native speakers in topic continuity context. The experimental groups showed some differences, related to the L1 – L2 combination and the level of proficiency in the L2. In particular, the performance of the L1 Spanish – L2 Greek speakers was more native-like than the performance of L1 Greek – L2 Spanish speakers. In the latter groups only, Margaza & Gavarró found an intermediate/advanced difference, with only intermediate learners differing significantly from native speakers. As the authors argue, this asymmetry (as well as the native-like performance of L2 Greek speakers, even at the intermediate level) is not predicted by either version of the Interface Hypothesis (Sorace & Filiaci 2006; Sorace 2011). As an explanation, the authors propose that if Greek defines a superset with respect to Spanish, Spanish learners of Greek will be less likely to accept a construction which is unacceptable in Greek, while the reverse is possible. Hence, assuming transfer to have a role, Greek learners of Spanish will be more likely to over-accept overt pronouns which are acceptable in Greek but not in Spanish.
2.4 Variation in the use and interpretation of null subjects: A view from Greek and Italian
Jacopo Torregrossa, Maria Andreou & Christiane Bongartz focus on the properties of pro in Greek and Italian, starting from the observation that a sentence like (2) (their ex. (4)) sounds quite unnatural in Italian, while its equivalent (3) (their ex. (3)) is perfectly natural in Greek, suggesting that a strong subject bias of pro holds in Italian but not in Greek:
- (2)
- Poiché
- Because
- Caterina
- c.F.SG.
- ha
- AUX.IND.PRES.3SG.
- severamente
- severely
- criticato
- criticize.PPT
- Mario,
- M.M.SG.,
- (pro)
- pro
- era
- be.IND.IMPF.3SG
- imbarazzato
- embarrassed.M.SG.
- (3)
- Epeidí
- Because
- i Caterina
- C.NOM.F.SG.
- epékrine
- criticize.IND.PF.3SG.
- afstirá
- severely
- ton Mario,
- M.ACC.M.SG.
- (pro)
- pro
- ítan
- be.IND.IMPF.3SG.
- ntropiasménos.
- embarrassed.NOM.M.SG.
The results of an interpretation task and a production task involving native Italian and Greek speakers confirm the observation: while all participants prefer to interpret or use a null subject in coreference with a subject antecedent, Greek participants allow coreference of a null subject with an object antecedent to a significantly greater extent than Italian participants. The authors interpret this difference as stemming from the fact that syntactic prominence of the antecedent is a more reliable cue for anaphora resolution in Italian than in Greek. According to Tsimpli & Roussou (2006), indeed, subjects and objects in Greek may occur in the same clitic-shell, contrary to Italian (which does not allow VSO in broad focus thetic sentences).
2.5 Null and overt subject pronouns in topic continuity and topic shift: An investigation of the narrative productions of Italian Natives, Greek Natives and near-native second language speakers of Italian with Greek as a first language
Similar findings concerning the use of pro in Greek and Italian are reported by Elisa Di Domenico, Ioli Baroncini & Andrea Capotorti. The authors analyze the kind of subject referring expression (null and overt pronouns as well as lexical DPs) produced in elicited narrations by the three groups of speakers in terms of topicality and number and kind of active referents. Their results show that while pro differs in the productions of Greek and Italian natives (in that it is significantly more attested in topic shift contexts in Greek than in Italian), L2 speakers of Italian do not transfer this property of Greek into their Italian. On the contrary, while overt pronouns are equally employed by Greek and Italian natives, L2 speakers of Italian over-use overt pronouns, using them also in topic continuity. This result, that is in line with previous literature, cannot be due to cross-linguistic influence (since Greek and Italian do not differ in the use of overt pronouns) nor to a difficulty to cope with topicality, since L2 speakers were able to do so in the case of pro.
Further analyzing the data, the authors found that over-use of overt pronouns in topic continuity is attested particularly when two referents which differ for gender and/or number are active.
As Arnold & Griffin (2007) have proposed, attention is a limited resource, and the presence of an additional character decreases referents’ activation as an effect of cognitive load. In this environment, as Di Domenico, Baroncini & Capotorti argue, the topic/non-topic distinction is less clear for L2ers, whose processing resources are more taxed (Sorace, 2016), and the overt pronoun is the sufficiently explicit (hence not over-explicit) device to employ for them.
2.6 Anaphora resolution and word-order across adulthood: Ageing effects on online listening comprehension
Georgia Fotiadou, Ana I. Pérez Muñoz & Ianthi M. Tsimpli investigate the role of ageing in the interpretation of two overt pronominal referring expressions in Greek (namely aftós ‘he’, and o ídhios ‘the same’) using online (visual world paradigm) and offline (comprehension questions) measures. A group of young (mean age 23.51) and a group of elderly (mean age 67.97) adult speakers participated in this experimental study. Working memory scores differed significantly between age groups, with a higher capacity in young adults. Another factor, manipulated in the experimental materials, is clause-internal word order (SVO vs. OVS, both possible in Greek) in the presentation of subject and object antecedents.7 On theoretical grounds, the authors assume that the interpretation of the subject pronoun o ídhios should reveal a subject preference in both word orders (since the authors assume that, due to its lexical meaning, its interpretation is driven by parallelism), while aftós should prefer an object antecedent particularly in the SVO order. Age differences are expected in online processing (related to cognitive decline in the older participants), but not in the off-line task. Results partly confirm the predictions, with no age-related differences in the off-line task: both groups preferred a subject interpretation for o ídhios, while preferences for aftós were at chance level, contrary to previous studies showing a clear object preference (e.g. Papadopoulou et al. 2015). In the on-line task, compared to aftós (where age-related differences were found in both word orders, with younger adults spending more time looking at the subject picture), the online processing of the pronoun o ídhios is preserved in elderly adults when structural complexity is lower (i.e., in SVO).
2.7 Children’s subjects in Clitic Left Dislocations in Italian
A clausal type analogous to the Greek OVS sentences employed by Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli, is studied by Claudia Manetti & Adriana Belletti, who examine the choice of subjects by 4-6 y.o. Italian children in this specific syntactic configuration (Clitic Left Dislocations- ClLDs - of the kind: Il gatto, il cane lo lava ‘The cat, the dog washes it’).8 In line with some of the previous literature, the authors assume that in the derivation of this structure a locality problem occurs: in its movement to the left peripheral topic position, the lexical DP object encounters an intervening subject. The type of subject modulates the type of intervention: if the subject is a lexical DP as well, we can have inclusion (if subject and object match for number) or intersection (if they mismatch for number). If the subject is pronominal, we have a disjunction relation. While the inclusion relation is too costly for pre-school children, the intersection relation can be mastered, and the disjunction relation is the easiest one (Friedmann et al. 2009, Belletti et al. 2012). The realization of subjects in ClLDs is compared to its realization in subject-clitic-verb structures (Il cane lo lava ‘The dog washes it’), both elicited by Belletti & Manetti (2019) through two picture description tasks in which children were asked to talk about the patient(s) of transitive verbs after hearing a patient-oriented question. In the ‘one patient condition’, the object topic need not be lexicalized (subject-clitic-verb structures are elicited), while in the ‘two patients condition’ the patient must be expressed, hence ClLDs were elicited. The elicitation of ClLDs included two conditions: a single agent vs. two agents. The authors find a significantly higher use of pro in ClLDs, in the ‘two agents condition’: plural pro, the authors argue, can have a generic interpretation. Hence its use allows children to avoid the lexical expression of the agent, determining a disjunction relation, which is the easiest intervention condition. Use of plural generic pro, thus, can be seen as a strategy to avoid a locality problem. Overt pronouns were never used, demonstrating that children at this age are well aware of the distribution of null and overt subject pronouns in Italian.
3. Conclusion
As we have seen, previous research on micro-variation in subject realization and interpretation in anaphoric contexts has brought to the fore a number of relevant research issues that concern the status of some subject anaphoric devices in null vs. non-null subject languages, the possibility of micro-variation among null subject languages, and the way in which populations of L2 speakers, elderly speakers, and children deal with the task at stake and the factors that may influence this process. Taken together, the studies in this collection not only shed light on many of these issues, but they also raise novel research questions that open new perspectives of investigation.
Gargiulo, Bernardini & Tronnier found that overt pronouns in Swedish are preferably interpreted as co-referent with a subject antecedent. This subject bias appears analogous to that described for English (Arnold 2010) and Dutch (Roberts et al. 2008; Hendriks et al. 2014) overt pronouns, suggesting that overt pronoun biases are quite uniform (and subject-oriented) in non-null subject languages. In null subject languages instead, overt pronouns are preferably strong, at slightly varying degrees depending on the language considered. Some of the studies in this collection indeed confirm micro-differences among null subject languages, not only for overt pronouns, but for null pronouns as well. pro in Greek seems to differ from pro in Italian in that the former has a less clear subject/topic bias in comprehension and is used to retrieve a non-subject/non-topic antecedent to a larger extent in elicited production (Torregrossa, Andreu & Bongartz; Di Domenico, Baroncini & Capotorti). A clear subject bias in Greek is instead displayed by the infrequent overt pronoun o ídhios, which Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli examine in comparison to aftós. Assuming that aftós has a more ambiguous nature than o ídhios,9 and hence is more complex, Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli show that elderly speakers diverge from younger speakers when complexity (ambiguity, in this case) increases, an effect that only shows up in on-line processing. A different kind of complexity, namely unclear prominence of the antecedent, seems to tax L2 speakers, as suggested by Contemori & Dussias results in comparison to Roberts et al.’s (2008), as well as by the results obtained by Di Domenico, Baroncini & Capotorti.
The idea that factors contributing to complexity may be different for different populations of speakers is also highlighted by Manetti & Belletti’s 4–6 y. o. children, who struggle with intervention effects. These children reveal as well that diverging patterns – in their case, the use of a plural pro subject with generic meaning – may not be determined by difficulties concerning the anaphoric devices per se, as these children appear to fully master the constraints regulating the null/overt option in Italian, a claim that could possibly be extended to elderly speakers and L2 speakers.
Patterns diverging from the baseline, furthermore, may not emerge if the task is relatively easy, as in off-line tasks and with non-ambiguous pronouns for elderly speakers (Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli). For L2 speakers, as we have seen, diverging patterns may not emerge in contexts in which prominence of an antecedent is clear, but also in the ‘superset’ language, as Margaza & Gavarró suggest.
Importantly, some studies in this collection show that L2 speakers of two null subject languages may indeed diverge from native speakers in anaphora resolution when the task/context is sufficiently complex. Cross-linguistic influence may play a role here, when, as Margaza & Gavarró show the two null subject languages of these speakers exhibit micro-differences.
Finally, in addition to the widespread and variegated micro-variation in subject realization and interpretation in anaphoric contexts brought to light by the studies in this collection, it is interesting to note that prosodic features may play the same role in otherwise unrelated languages. This is suggested by the fact that both speakers of a null subject language (Italian) and speakers of a non-null subject language (Swedish) use the same prosody (a longer inter-clausal pause and a more prominent pronunciation of the pronoun) to signal the non-preferred interpretation of an overt pronoun, as found by Gargiulo, Bernardini & Tronnier.
List of abbreviations
3 = third person, ACC= accusative, F = feminine, IMPF = imperfect, IND = indicative, M = masculine, NOM = nominative, PPT = past participle, PRES = present, REFL = reflexive, SG = singular.
Notes
- Arnold (2010) calls this preference of the English overt pronoun the ‘first-mention bias’. According to earlier studies which have interpreted this effect as a ‘parallel function strategy’ (Sheldon 1974), if the English overt pronoun bears stress, it is instead interpreted as co-referent with an object antecedent. This does not necessarily entail, however, that the stressed English overt pronoun is the equivalent of the Italian overt pronoun. See Carminati (2002: Ch.1) for some discussion. [^]
- In Cardinaletti & Starke’s (1999) terms, the English overt pronoun may be weak or strong (Cardinaletti 2004a and b), while the Italian overt pronoun is a strong pronoun. Cardinaletti (2004a and b) and Frascarelli (2007) signal however an ongoing change in Italian, whereby weak overt pronouns are also attested, mainly in informal contexts. [^]
- Rizzi (2018) assumes that this is so because c-command is not involved in these cases. When two possible antecedents for pro in the preceding clause c-command the null pronoun, there is no clear preference. In (i), for instance (Rizzi 2018: 516), pro can equally co-refer with either Francesca or Maria:
- (i)
- Francesca
- Francesca
- ha
- has
- fatto
- made
- notare
- notice
- a
- to
- Maria
- Maria
- che
- that
- pro
- pro
- era
- was
- molto
- very
- stanca
- tired.FS
- ‘Francesca made Maria realize that she was very tired’
- The relevant sentences were in the main-subordinate order with antecedents in the main clause, such as (i.a) in Sorace & Filiaci (2006) and (i.b) in Chamorro (2018):
- (i.a)
- La
- the
- madre
- mother
- dà
- gives
- un
- a
- bacio
- kiss
- alla
- to-the
- figlia
- daughter
- mentre
- while
- pro/lei
- pro/she
- si
- REFLsi
- mette
- puts
- il
- the
- cappotto
- coat
- ‘The mother gives a kiss to her daughter while she puts on her coat’
- (i.b)
- La
- the
- madre
- mother
- saludó
- greeted.SG
- a
- to
- la
- the
- chica
- girl
- cuando
- when
- ella
- she
- cruzaba
- crossed.SG
- una
- a
- calle
- street
- con
- with
- mucho
- much
- tráfico
- traffic
- ‘The mother greeted the girl when she crossed a street with a lot of traffic.’
- It is worth mentioning, however, that in the comprehension study Giannakou & Sitaridou (2020) used main-subordinate sentences with the antecedents in the main clause As we have seen (footnote 4 above), in this syntactic environment the subject bias of pro is weaker in Italian as well. [^]
- As Contemori & Dussias underline, there are two main reasons why the experimental materials, and the related task, in their study (exemplified in (i)) were less complex for L2 speakers than the ones employed by Roberts et al. (2008) (exemplified in (ii)). The first is that their sentences (like those employed by Cunnings et al. 2017) were not globally ambiguous, the second is that in Roberts et al.’s materials the two referents introduced in the discourse had similar degrees of saliency:
(i) A builder saw a doctor by the door. He briefly thanked the doctor/builder for his help The accessibility of the referents, they conclude, is an important factor in pronoun resolution. [^](ii) Hans and Peter are in the office. While Peter is working, he is eating a sandwich. - OVS structures employed in this study were structures in which the object is topicalized and resumed by a clitic pronoun (Clitic Left Dislocations), as in (i), Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli’s ex. (2.b):
- (i)
- Ton
- the.ACC
- Kosta
- Kostas.ACC
- ton
- him
- sinandise
- met.3SG.
- o
- the.NOM
- Janis
- Janis.NOM
- ‘Kostas, Janis met him’
- It is worth noting that in these structures the subject is post-verbal in Greek (as we have seen in fn.6 above), while it is pre-verbal in Italian. See Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli (this collection) for some discussion. [^]
- Fotiadou, Pérez Muñoz & Tsimpli unexpectedly found, for aftós, chance level preferences for subject and object antecedents. A similar, unexpected result concerning aftós is found by Margaza & Gavarró, in that the Greek native speakers they examined accepted it in topic continuity contexts at a rate which was higher than the acceptance rate of the Spanish speakers for the overt pronoun él in the same contexts. Di Domenico, Baroncini & Capotorti found instead that aftós is almost exclusively used in topic shift contexts by Greek natives in their narratives. These partly conflicting results suggest, also in the light of the results from previous literature, that the antecedent retrieval properties of aftós deserve further investigation. [^]
Competing Interests
The authors have no competing interests to declare.
References
Ariel, Mira. 1990. Accessing noun phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.
Ariel, Mira. 2001. Accessibility theory: an overview. In Ted J. M. Sanders, Joost Schilperoord & Wilbert Spooren (eds.), Text Representation: Linguistic and Psycholinguistic aspects, 29–87. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/hcp.8.04ari
Arnold, Jennifer. 2010. How Speakers Refer: The Role of Accessibility. Language and Linguistics Compass 4. 187–203. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00193.x
Arnold, Jennifer, Janet Eisenband, Sarah Brown-Schmidt & John Trueswell. 2000. The immediate use of gender information: eye-tracking evidence of the time-course of pronoun resolution. Cognition 76. 13–26. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00073-1
Arnold, Jennifer E. & Zenzi M. Griffin. 2007. The effect of additional characters on choice of referring expression: Everyone counts. Journal of Memory and Language 56. 521–536. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.007
Belletti, Adriana. 2001. ‘Inversion’ as focalization. In Aafke Hulk & Jean Yves Pollock (eds.), Subject Inversion and the theory of Universal Grammar, 60–90. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Belletti, Adriana. 2004. Aspects of the low IP area. In Luigi Rizzi (ed.), The cartography of Syntactic Structures vol. II: The structure of CP and IP, 16–51. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Belletti, Adriana & Claudia Manetti. 2019. Topics and passives in Italian-speaking children and adults. Language acquisition 26(2). 153–182. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2018.1508465
Belletti, Adriana, Elisa Bennati & Antonella Sorace. 2007. Theoretical and developmental issues in the syntax of subjects: evidence from near- native Italian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 25. 657–689. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-007-9026-9
Belletti, Adriana, Naama Friedmann, Dominique Brunato & Luigi Rizzi. 2012. Does gender make a difference? Comparing the effect of gender on children’s comprehension of relative clauses in Hebrew and Italian. Lingua 122(10). 1053–1069. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.02.007
Calabrese, Andrea. 1986. Pronomina. In Naoki Fukui, Tova R. Rapoport & Elizabeth Sagey (eds.), Papers in Theoretical Linguistics, 1–46. Cambridge, MA: MITWPL.
Cardinaletti, Anna. 2004a. La traduzione dei pronomi: interferenza sintattica e cambiamento linguistico. In Giuliana Garzone & Anna Cardinaletti (eds.), Lingua, mediazione linguistica e interferenza, 129–150. Milano: Franco Angeli.
Cardinaletti, Anna. 2004b. Toward a Cartography of Subject Positions. In Luigi Rizzi (ed.), The Structure of CP and IP. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures 2. 115–165. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Cardinaletti, Anna & Michal Starke. 1999. The typology of structural deficiency: A case study of the three classes of pronouns. In Henk van Riemsdijk (ed.), Clitics in the Languages of Europe, 145–234. Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110804010.145
Carminati, Maria Nella. 2002. The processing of Italian subject pronouns. Amherst, MA: University of Massachusetts dissertation.
Chamorro, Gloria. 2018. Offline interpretation of subject pronouns by native speakers of Spanish. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 3(1). 27. DOI: http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.256
Contemori, Carla, Ohood Asiri & Elva Deida Perea Irigoyen. 2019. Anaphora resolution in L2 English: an analysis of discourse complexity and cross-linguistic interference. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 41(5). 971–998. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000111
Cunnings, Ian, Georgia Fotiadou & Ianthi Tsimpli. 2017. Anaphora resolution and reanalysis during L2 sentence processing: evidence from the visual world paradigm. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 39(4). 621–652. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000292
Fedele, Emily & Elsi Kaiser. 2014. Looking back and looking forward: Anaphora and cataphora in Italian. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 20(1). 10. 79–90. http://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol20/iss1/10
Filiaci, Francesca. 2010. Null and overt subject biases in Spanish and Italian: a cross-linguistic comparison. In Claudia Borgonovo, Miriam Espanol-Echevarria & Philip Prèvost (eds.), Selected proceedings of the 12th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium, 171–182. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Filiaci, Francesca, Antonella Sorace & Manuel Carreiras. 2014. Anaphoric biases of null and overt subjects in Italian and Spanish: a cross- linguistic comparison. Language, Cognition and Neurosciences 28(7). 825–843. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.801502
Frascarelli, Mara. 2007. Subjects, Topics and the Interpretation of Referential pro. An interface approach to the linking of (null) pronouns. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 25. 691–734. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-007-9025-x
Friedmann, Naama, Adriana Belletti & Luigi Rizzi. 2009. Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua 119(1). 67–88. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.09.002
Giannakou, Aretousa & Ioanna Sitaridou. 2020. Microparametric variation in the syntax of Spanish and Greek pronominal subjects. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics 5(1). 75. 1–42. DOI: http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.960
Gilligan, Gary. 1987. A cross-linguistic approach to the pro-drop parameter. Los Angeles, CA: University of Southern California dissertation.
Hendriks, Henriëtte. 2003. The use of nouns in reference maintenance: the seeming contradiction in adult second language acquisition. In Anna Giacalone Ramat (ed.), Typology and Second Language Acquisition, 291–326. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110891249.291
Hendriks, Petra, Charlotte Koster & John C. J. Hoeks. 2014. Referential choices across the lifespan: Why children and elderly adults produce ambiguous pronouns. Language, cognition and neuroscience 29(4). 391–407. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.766356
Lozano, Cristόbal. 2006. The development of the syntax-information structure interface: Greek learners of Spanish. In Vincent Torrens & Linda Escobar (eds.), The acquisition of syntax in Romance languages, 371–399. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/lald.41.18loz
Margaza, Panagiota & Aurora Bel. 2006. Null subjects at the syntax-pragmatics interface: Evidence from Spanish interlanguage of Greek speakers. In Mary Grantham O’Brien, Christine Shea & John Archibald (eds.), Proceedings of GASLA 2006, 88–97. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Ordóñez, Francisco. 2007. Cartography of post-verbal subjects in Spanish and Catalan. In Sergio Baauw, Frank Drijkoningen & Manuela Pinto (eds.), Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2005, 259–280. Amsterdam: Benjamins. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.291.17ord
Papadopoulou, Despina, Eleni Peristeri, Evagelia Plemenou, Theodoros Marinis & Ianthi M. Tsimpli. 2015. Pronoun ambiguity resolution in Greek: Evidence from monolinguals adults and children. Lingua 155. 98–120. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2014.09.006
Pesetsky, David. 1995. Zero Syntax: Experiencers and Cascades. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Rizzi, Luigi. 1982. Issues in Italian Syntax. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110883718
Rizzi, Luigi. 2005. On some properties of subjects and topics. In Laura Brugè, Giuliana Giusti, Nicola Munaro, Walter Schweikert & Giuseppina Turano (eds.), Proceedings of the XXX Incontro di Grammatica Generativa, 203–224. Venezia: Cafoscarina.
Rizzi, Luigi. 2018. Subjects, topics and the interpretation of pro. In Petrosino, Roberto, Pietro Cerrone & Harry van der Hulst (eds.), From Sounds to Structures: Beyond the Veil of Maya, 510–529. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1515/9781501506734-019
Roberts, Leah, Marianne Gullberg & Peter Indefrey. 2008. Online pronoun resolution in L2 discourse: L1 influence and general learner effects. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 30. 333–357. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263108080480
Roussou, Anna & Ianthi M. Tsimpli. 2006. On Greek VSO again! Journal of Linguistics 42. 317–344. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226706003914
Serratrice, Ludovica, Antonella Sorace & Sandra Paoli. 2004. Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax- pragmatics interface: subjects and objects in Italian- English bilingual and monolingual acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7(3). 183–206. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728904001610
Sheldon, Amy. 1974. The role of parallel function in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 3(3). 272–281. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80064-2
Sorace, Antonella. 2011. Pinning down the concept of ‘interface’ in bilinguals. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 1(1). 1–33. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.1.01sor
Sorace, Antonella. 2016. Referring expressions and executive functions in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6(5). 669–684. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.15055.sor
Sorace, Antonella & Francesca Filiaci. 2006. Anaphora resolution in near- native speakers of Italian. Second Language Research 22(3). 339–368. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1191/0267658306sr271oa
Sorace, Antonella, Ludovica Serratrice, Francesca Filiaci & Michela Baldo. 2009. Discourse conditions in subject pronoun realization: Testing the linguistic intuitions of older bilingual children. Lingua 119(3). 460–477. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.09.008
Taraldsen, Tarald. 1980. On the Nominative Island Constraint, vacuous application and the that – trace filter. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Linguistic Club.
Tsimpli, Ianthi M. & Antonella Sorace. 2006. Differentiating interfaces: L2 performance in syntax- semantics and syntax-discourse phenomena. In David Bamman, Tatiana Magnitzkaia & Colleen Zaller (eds.) Proceedings of the 30th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 653–664. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
Tsimpli, Ianthi M., Antonella Sorace, Caroline Heycock & Francesca Filiaci. 2004. First language attrition and syntactic subjects: a study of Greek and Italian near native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism 8(3). 257–277. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1177/13670069040080030601
van Rij, Jacolien, Hedderik van Rijn & Petra Hendriks. 2013. How WM load influences linguistic processing in adults: A Computational Model of Pronoun interpretation in discourse. Topics in cognitive science 5(3). 564–580. DOI: http://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12029