1 Introduction

Since the onset of language research, one particular model has dominated the field: that of the monolingual speaker. Historically, monolinguals were viewed as the ‘baseline’ language user, while bilinguals were defined in relation to their simpler counterparts, notably as being “two monolinguals in one person” (see Grosjean 1989 for a discussion). While it is reasonable for science to first focus on less complex cases (i.e., monolinguals), global numbers indicate that monolinguals may not be a baseline after all. In fact, it is estimated that over half of the world’s population speaks more than one language (Grosjean & Miller 1994), making bilingualism and multilingualism the norm rather than the exception.

With the development of the field, more and more interdisciplinary experts now agree that the bilingual mind may be distinct from that of the monolingual, simply by virtue of holding two languages within one single cognitive system (e.g., MacSwan 2000). For instance, a wealth of data indicates that many bilinguals possess cooperative lexicons (e.g., Beauvillain & Grainger 1987; Hartsuiker, Pickering & Veltkamp 2004; Sabourin, Brien & Burkholder 2014), seemingly integrated within at least one linguistic interface. Despite this upsurge in empirical interest, the underlying composition of the bilingual language system is still obscure. Fortunately, one practice may illuminate its particulars: code-switching.

Code-switching involves the “alternation of two languages within a single discourse, sentence or constituent” (Poplack 1980: 583), as in (1). The practice is rule-governed and systematic, rather than an indication of linguistic deficiency (see Heredia & Altarriba 2001). This means that code-switchers possess community-specific (e.g., Adamou & Shen 2019) intuitive judgements about mixed sentences (e.g., (1b) is plainly unacceptable).

    1.  
    1. a.
    1.   Los
    2.   the
    1. ciudadanos
    2. citizens
    1. están
    2. are
    1. supporting
    2. supporting
    1. the
    2. the
    1. program.
    2. program
    1.    ‘The citizens are supporting the program.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *You
    2.   you
    1. have
    2. have
    1. contado
    2. told
    1. esa
    2. that
    1. historia.
    2. story
    1.    ‘You have told that story.’

Such consistent acceptability judgements offer a glimpse into the underlying bilingual system. They reveal the ways in which languages can and cannot be mixed, and therefore, shed light on the possible cognitive associations between the code-switched languages at each linguistic interface (e.g., phonology, morphology, syntax). In short, the examination of code-switched utterances allows linguists to model the bilingual language faculty while considering the bilingual as a baseline, rather than the monolingual.

As such, the current paper utilizes code-switching as a mechanism to assess previously purported models of bilingual cognition. In particular, the underlying structure of the bilingual language faculty is probed by two main questions: How many phonological systems are employed for code-switched utterances? How many syntactic subsets characterize code-switched sentences? Evidence both empirical (i.e., corpus-based data analyses) and theoretical in nature confirms that code-switching is derived from a unique syntactic grammar but dual phonologies, as past models have suggested (MacSwan 2000; MacSwan & Colina 2014).

The current paper is structured as follows: the next section expands on background concepts relative to code-switching and the bilingual language faculty under a minimalist perspective. Section 3 is devoted to the interface of phonology. Here, I analyze syllabic structure frequencies by using a Spanish-English code-switching corpus (the Bangor Miami Corpus; Deuchar, Davies, Herring, Parafita Couto & Carter 2014) in order to tap into the underlying phonological code-switching systems. In the fourth section, the interface of syntax is addressed; I expand on the theoretical support for the singularity of the syntactic code-switching grammar and once again utilize the code-switching corpus as empirical support. The final section concludes.

2 Background

2.1 Code-switching and the bilingual language faculty

As has been stated, code-switching consists of the juxtaposition of multiple languages within a single speech event. In many communities, code-switching is regarded as typical in-group linguistic behaviour (e.g., Poplack 1980; Poplack 1988; González-Vilbazo & López 2011; Adamou & Shen 2019; Gosselin & Sabourin 2021; etc.). Switching can occur both inter-sententially (between sentences) or intra-sententially (within sentences); the use of the intra-sentential code-switching has been linked to high proficiency or balanced language-skills among speakers (e.g., Poplack 1980; Koban 2013). The examples in (3) (from Valdés Kroff, Dussias, Gerfen, Perrotti & Bajo 2017) depict both switching types, respectively.

    1. (2)
    1. Spanish/English
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Ayer
    2. yesterday
    1. fui
    2. went
    1. al
    2. to the
    1. supermercado.
    2. supermarket
    1. I bought some apples.
    2. I bought some apples
    1. ‘Yesterday I went to the supermarket.
    2. I bought some apples.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. El
    2. the
    1. niño
    2. boy
    1. está
    2. is
    1. reading
    2. reading
    1. the
    2. the
    1. book.
    2. book
    1. ‘The boy is reading the book.’

As researchers recognized the systematicity of code-switches, a multitude of (primarily syntactic) language mixing constraints were introduced. For instance, the Equivalence Constraint (3), brought forth by Poplack (1980), claimed that code-switching boundaries are prohibited where such a switch may give rise to an illicit word-order in one of the two languages. Following the Equivalence Constraint, a series of language mixing constraints were proposed: the Closed class constraint (Joshi 1982), the Government constraint (Di Sciullo, Muysken & Singh 1986), the Matrix language frame (Myers-Scotton 1997), the Functional head constraint (Belazi et al. 1994), among others. While these theories often possess reasonable predictive power, most have been critiqued by proponents of the constraint-free approach (e.g., MacSwan 2000; 2014; MacSwan & Colina 2014). This is because the Equivalence Constraint and many of its successors directly refer to the act of code-switching and therefore impose upon it an otherwise vacuous ‘third grammar’. By contrast, constraint-free formalism maintains that code-switching does not (and should not) possess constraints that are independent from monolingual speech. As it turns out, Chomsky’s minimalism is well equipped to carry out a constraint-free analysis of code-switching (MacSwan 2000; 2014).

(3) Equivalence Constraint: Code-switches will tend to occur at points in discourse where juxtaposition of Ll and L2 elements does not violate a syntactic rule of either language, i.e. at points around which the surface structures of the two languages map onto each other. (Poplack 1980: 586)

In minimalism, patterns observed from linguistic data are preferentially elucidated by the most simple and elegant assumptions. Consider Chomsky’s generally accepted hypothesis that the (monolingual) human language faculty is made up of A) a lexicon and B) a computational system (CHL) (see Chomsky 1995). In brief, CHL is responsible for Selecting lexical arrays from the lexicon, combining them into a syntactic structure with the operation Merge, and then Spelling-Out the derivation to logical form (LF: the interpretative level of language) and phonological form (PF: the sensorimotor aspect of language).

Adopting the constraint-free approach for code-switching data, MacSwan (2000) extended Chomsky’s minimalistic view of the human language (see Figure 1, left panel) to the bilingual language faculty. The bilingual version of the model (see Figure 1, right panel) includes two differences. First, there are two lexicons available to the bilingual speaker. Second, bilinguals possess discrete phonological systems, which are compartmentalized before PF. Just like the monolingual language faculty, however, bilinguals are thought to construct sentences within a single syntactic component.

Figure 1
Figure 1

Left: The (monolingual) language faculty, according to Chomsky (1995). Right: MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty (both adapted from MacSwan 2000; 2016).

This system very simply accounts for code-switching while avoiding switching-specific grammars: in the bilingual language faculty, CHL can select lexical items from multiple lexicons and combine them into a single syntactic construction. On the other hand, a bilingual individual can easily speak in a single language by selecting items from a single lexicon instead of two. Thus, by adopting a minimalist framework, MacSwan convincingly illustrated that an adequate code-switching grammar should not explicitly or conceptually refer to the act of using multiple languages (an insight previously broached by Mahootian 1993; Belazi et al. 1994). Instead, his model is anchored in the following assumptions (MacSwan 2000: 43):

(4) i. […] All grammatical relations and operations which are relevant to monolingual language are relevant to bilingual language, and only these.
  ii. Nothing constrains code switching apart from the requirements of the mixed grammars.

In short, assuming that two languages, Lx and Ly, are involved in a mixed sentence, and that these languages each possess their own grammar, Gx and Gy, any code-switched sentence is determined by GxGy. We do not need to know the arbitrary identity of any L, and we do not need to generate a switching-specific grammar.

Once the syntactic structure (the overt component) is derived, it must surface as a phonological/phonetic form. The monolingual model simply sends the sensorimotor elements of the derivation to the unique phonological system in the configuration. The bilingual language faculty, on the other hand, possesses more than one lexical set, and thus, more than one phonological system. In a monolingual utterance, bilinguals can easily respect the phonology of any of their languages: items from each lexicon are merely rendered to their corresponding phonological system. Contrastively, during code-switching, all lexical items must be parsed by their appropriate phonological systems after Spell-Out.1 Hence, to give rise to a felicitous code-switched speech segment, we assume that the phonological systems (PS) within the bilingual architecture are sequestered (PSx, PSy) before PF. Note that this architecture starkly contrasts other (primarily non-lexicalist) models (see also González-Vilbazo & López 2012; Alexiadou, Lohndal, Åfarli & Grimstad 2015; Alexiadou & Lohndal 2018; Stefanich, Cabrelli, Hilderman & Archibald 2019; etc.) which posit that bilinguals possess fully integrated grammars, including at the level of phonology (López 2020).

To recapitulate, MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty implies that code-switching is possible at the syntactic level, but that code-switching at PF is unavailable. This means that each segment involved in the switch respects the phonology of the lexical set it originates from, and furthermore, that there is no singular alternate phonological grammar that can account for the mixed utterance in its entirety. Presently, it may be asked exactly why the syntactic and phonological interfaces, by nature, differ within the architecture of the bilingual system. That is, what motivated MacSwan to posit that syntactic grammars can be combined, while the phonological counterparts could not? Essentially, this conjecture is based on the computational features of each interface—regardless of whether the system is monolingual or bilingual.

Within the syntactic interface, operations can take place at any given time. For instance, inserting a post-nominal adjective into a derivation before introducing a noun will still give rise to a correct surface form, as the lexical item is labeled with [adj. +post-nom.]. Based on this imperviousness to underlying order relations, there is no concern in stipulating that items from code-switched sentences are syntactically derived from an assembled set (i.e., a single lexical array constructed by CHL).

On the other hand, phonological rules or constraints have been noted as being order-dependent at least since Bromberger and Halle (1989), who penned the Principle in (5). Since phonological grammars are sensitive to ordering, it would not be parsimonious to assume that code-switching is characterized by the united phonologies of the languages involved in the switch. Consider, for instance, a set of phonological rules {R1, R2, R3}. In PSx of Lx, R1>R2,R3; in PSy of Ly, R1,R3>R2. What would take place if Spell-Out introduced a code-switched sentence composed of Lx and Ly into a combined PS? At the very least, the result of such a computation is challenging to obtain (though see Chapter 6 of López 2020 for a potential analysis).

(5) Phonological rules are ordered with respect to one another. A phonological rule R does not apply necessarily to the underlying representation; rather, R applies to the derived representation that results from the application of each applicable rule preceding R in the order of the rules (Bromberger & Halle 1989: 58–59).

Thus, the separation of the phonological systems within the architecture of the bilingual mind directly falls from the “imperfection” of phonology, notably its poor computational properties (Chomsky 2002: 118). It is a consequence of the order-specific nature of the phonological interface; if phonological systems were united in the model, their respective orders could be compromised. As strikingly noted by MacSwan & Colina (2014: 188), this entails that “language switching is phonological switching”, and nothing more. Such an observation falls into line with conceptualizations of language which claim that variation is confined to the externalization of language (Chomsky, Gallego & Ott 2019), while the internal language remains largely invariant.

The PF Interface Condition (PFIC; previously the PF Disjunction Theorem; MacSwan 2000: 45), spelled out in (6), accounts for the separation of the bilingual’s phonologies as per the computational properties of this interface (MacSwan & Colina 2014: 191). MacSwan and Colina assert (6.iv) that phonological switching is prohibited within words but not between words. This behaviour is based on the following assumption: on the way to PF, sensorimotor material is minimally parsed at the word level (see MacSwan & Colina 2014 for a discussion). Recent data does in fact indicate that word-internal phonological switching is virtually unattested, even if an acceptable morphological switch has occurred (Bessett 2017; see also MacSwan & Colina 2014; Stefanich & Cabrelli Amaro 2018).

(6) PF Interface Condition
  i. Phonological input is mapped to the output in one step with no intermediate representations.
  ii. Each set of internally ranked constraints is a constraint dominance hierarchy, and a language-particular phonology is a set of constraint dominance hierarchies.
  iii. Bilinguals have a separately encapsulated phonological system for each language in their repertoire in order to avoid ranking paradoxes, which result from the availability of distinct constraint dominance hierarchies with conflicting priorities.
  iv. Every syntactic head (word) must be phonologically parsed at Spell-Out. Therefore, the boundary between heads represents the minimal opportunity for code-switching.

Before continuing, it is important to distinguish code-switching from nonce-borrowing (and attested loanwords). According to original theory, code-switching, whether inter- or intra-sentential, involves unique processes (e.g., Grosjean 1995). Unlike switched segments, borrowed words are said to be morphologically, and often phonologically integrated into the target language (Poplack & Meechan 1998; though see Poplack, et al. 2020 for recent data). Since borrowings (and not code-switches) may be parsed by the phonological system of the recipient language (e.g., ‘rid-er’ is pronounced as French-like /ʁɑ͡ɪde/, even though it originates from the English riding /ɹɑ͡ɪdɪŋ/), these types of items do not give us much insight into the architecture of the bilingual’s phonological systems. From a computational perspective, sentences featuring borrowed words or loan words are not at all different from single-language speech; these items will not be considered in the empirical analyses of this paper.

In the subsequent section, past phonetic studies examining code-switching will be reviewed; these indirectly support MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty.

2.2 Experimental phonetic/phonological literature

A moderately large literature has investigated whether bilinguals abruptly switch between the phonetics of their languages when they produce mixed utterances, or alternatively, whether the phonetic systems somehow converge towards a new schema that is dissimilar from that of both languages in unilingual contexts. The results from these studies remain conflicting.

Early findings examining the voice-onset time (VOT) of French-English code-switchers indicate that bilinguals shift from one phonetic system to the next, with absolutely no phonetic remnant of the opposite language after a switching boundary (Grosjean & Miller 1994). These results have seldom been replicated (though see Brown 2015; Olson 2013 (bilingual contexts) for comparable conclusions). For the most part, studies have observed some sort of effect between the languages involved in the code-switch.

On one hand, some research indicates that code-switched speech may actually be hyper-articulated. This result has primarily been found for suprasegmental features, such as pitch (Olson 2012; see also Olson & Ortega-Llebaria 2010) and stress, as indicated by vowel length (Olson, 2012; Muldner et al. 2019). In other words, the suprasegmental features of the languages involved in the switch may diverge from each other, possibly in order to ensure maximal distance between their acoustic features. Hyper-articulation of code-switched items may also occur since switches are said to be “less predictable” (Olson 2012: 451) than single-language speech (see Bell, Brenier, Gregory, Girand & Jurafsky 2009 for supporting results). In any case, hyper-articulation is not problematic to MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty; it still presupposes that bilinguals have encapsulated phonological systems and that code-switches are not phonologically integrated.

On the other hand, several studies indicate that phonetic convergence does indeed take place between languages involved in a code-switch; this is by far the most common finding (Bullock, Toribio, Davis & Botero 2004; Bullock, Toribio, González & Dalola 2006; Antoniou, Best, Tyler & Kroos 2011; Balukas & Koops 2015; Piccinini & Arvanti 2015; López 2012; Ojeda, de Prada Pérez & Wayland 2018). For instance, when participants produced switches between Spanish and English, the VOT values of stops in these languages may shift towards each other. Interestingly, this shift is often asymmetric: it is primarily observed within the language that contains a broader VOT spectrum. Thus, English VOT may shift towards shorter Spanish-like values, since these segments can afford to shift while respecting their own phonological thresholds (Bullock et al. 2006; Balukas & Koops 2015; Piccinini & Arviniti 2015).

Despite these complex and often antithetical findings, one result remains unchanged: the presence of phonetic convergence does not result in a merge of the systems. In fact, the phonetic shift never extends as far as the respective phonological categorization boundaries of each language. For example, Balukas and Koops (2015) observed a durational shift for English voiceless stops in code-switched compared to monolingual speech, notably towards shorter Spanish-like VOT values. Nevertheless, the VOTs of the code-switched English stops still remained within the typical English range (see also Bullock et al. 2004; 2006; Antoniou et al. 2011; Piccinini & Arvaniti 2015, etc.). This means that the often-observed phonetic convergence is, as its name implies, simply phonetic in nature. It does not negate the duality of the phonological systems according MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty.

In brief, the acoustic literature illustrates that phonetic convergence (among other possibilities) is available for code-switches, but that this shift is virtually never phonological. These results rather support the notion that code-switching does not impact the phonologies of the languages involved in the mixed utterances. Up to this point, however, mostly indirect evidence concerning this matter has been examined. In the subsequent section, the duality of the phonological systems, as postulated by MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty, is assessed. Unlike previous research, this is done by examining a facet that is quite distinct from the phonetic system: that of syllabic structure.

3 Phonology: Divorced but co-parenting

3.1 Background

The goal of the current section is to investigate the underlying phonological system of code-switched utterances, in order to verify MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty. One way of tapping into these rather abstract notions is by comparing code-switched languages which differ within a specific aspect of their respective phonological systems. For instance, MacSwan and Colina (2014) utilized the Spanish-specific phonological rule resulting in the realization of intervocalic stops as approximants. Unilingual English displays no pattern similar to the Spanish approximant rule. These authors observed that in Spanish-English code-switches favouring the application of the Spanish rule (e.g., /migowst/ ‘mi ghost’ → */miɰowst/), stops were only realized as approximants in about 10% of the recordings (compared to 93% in unilingual Spanish). However, phonological processes were successfully triggered for segments belonging to the same language in which the process was active (e.g., Spanish voicing rule: /misgowsts/ ‘mis ghosts’ → /mizgowst/). MacSwan and Colina thus cleverly demonstrated that code-switching boundaries involve an abrupt shift in the bilingual’s phonological systems.

In the current paper, I utilize the phonotactic structure (i.e., the permissibility of certain phoneme combinations and not others; Anttila 2008) of Spanish-English code-switched speech in order to garner insight into the phonological system(s) that is—or are—employed during mixed-language utterances. The majority of the research substantiates the idea that language-specific phonotactic patterns are psychologically real (e.g., Friederici & Wessels 1993; Jusczyk, Friederici, Wessels, Svenkerud & Jusczyk 1993; Jusczyk, Luce & Charles-Luce 1994), and that they arise from speakers’ underlying grammar (Coleman & Pierrehumbert 1997; Frisch, Large & Pisoni 2000; Shademan 2006; Albright 2008; though see Bailey & Hahn 2001; Hammond 2004). This means that examining such patterns effectively allow us to hone into the phonological component of the language faculty, as modelled by Chomsky and MacSwan.

The possible empirical outcomes are presented in (7). Outcome A) supports phonological encapsulation as it is posited in current model of the bilingual language faculty (MacSwan 2000); outcome B) does not support MacSwan’s model. Rather, outcome B) may indicate a degree of integration for bilinguals’ phonological systems.

(7) Potential empirical outcomes: Let a code-switch be composed of x and y
  A. If x respects the phonotactics of Lx, and y respects the phonotactics of Ly, the phonologies of Lx and Ly are impermeable.
  B. Alternatively, if x does not respect the phonotactics of Lx, or y does not respect the phonotactics of Ly the phonologies of Lx and Ly are permeable.

The general phonotactic differences between Spanish and English are exploited in order to test the outcomes in (7). Spanish and English have distinct constraints regarding the legality of certain syllabic structures. English has a relatively lax syllable structure; it can accommodate up to three consonants in the onset position, and even more in the coda (e.g. strengths /strɛŋ(k)θs/ ➔ CCCVC(C)CC, glimpsed /ɡlɪmpst/ ➔ CCVCCCC). Contrastively, consonant clusters within a given syllable are greatly disfavoured in Spanish (e.g., Barlow 2003). As can be seen in Table 1, this is reflected in the relative frequency of syllable types in unilingual English (Delattre & Olsen 1969) and Spanish (Villanueva-Reyes 2013).

Table 1

Proportion of syllable types reported in previous literature for unilingual English2 (Delattre & Olsen 1969) and Spanish3 (Villanueva-Reyes 2013) language.

Syllable type English Spanish Syllable type English Spanish
CV 27.6 55.82 CVCC 6.7 0.02
CVC 31.9 21.6 CCVCC 1.3 0.01
V 8.1 9.58 CVCCC 0.3
VC 11.9 8.34 CCVCCC 0.2
CCV 4 3.14 VCCC 0.1
VCC 2.9 0.13 CCCVC 0.1
CCVC 4.8 0.06 CCCV 0.05

As it turns out, the grammatical distinction between English and Spanish is easily modeled with the Optimality Theory framework (OT). In OT (see Smolensky & Prince 1993), the ranking of universal constraints forms a language-specific grammar; the numerous permutations for hierarchical constraint rankings give rise to cross-linguistic differences in phonological systems. When comparing languages with different constraints rankings (and thus different superficial phonological attributes), one can use OT to determine which system is currently active in code-switched discourse.

In the present study, the relevant constraint which primarily accounts for the phonological differences between Spanish and English is *Complex (i.e., “Assign a violation for every syllable with a consonant cluster”). That is, *Complex possesses a distinct ranking in Spanish and English, being of a relative higher rank in the former than the latter (8). This simple distinction accounts for the resulting phonotactics of both languages, as can be seen in Tables 2 and 3 (loosely adapted from Misker & Anderson 2003).

Table 2

English syllabification of ‘absorbed’.

/absorb-d/ Dep.IO Peak Max.IO *Complex
a) ab.zorbd *
b) ab.zord *! *
c) ab.zor.bed *!
d) ab.zor.bd *!
Table 3

Spanish syllabification of absorb-to (‘absorbed’).

/absorb-to/ Dep.IO Peak *Complex Max.IO
a) ab.sor.to *
b) ab.sorb.to *!
c) ab.sor.be.to *!
d) ab.sor.b.to *!

In English (Table 2), *Complex is ranked lower than Dep-IO (i.e., ‘No insertion’), Peak (‘Syllables must contain vowels’) and Max-IO (‘No deletion’). Consequently, an input like /abzorbd/ ‘absorbed’ is syllabified into two units: a VC syllable, and a complex CVCCC syllable. Simply put, this pattern is obtained since English grammar ‘prefers’ outputs that preserve all of their original segments (i.e., faithful candidates), rather than outputs without consonant clusters. Crucially, this preference is determined by the outranking of Max.IO over *Complex (8a).

Contrastively, in Spanish (Table 3), an input such as /absorb-to/ ‘absorbed’ is syllabified into three units, none of which include a consonant cluster: VC, CVC and CV syllables. Syllabification occurs in this manner since consonant clusters are less desirable in Spanish than deleted segments, as determined by the ranking of * Complex over Max-IO (8b).

(8) Crucial constraint rankings of interest:
  a. English: Max-IO > *Complex
  b. Spanish: *Complex > Max-IO

With only a handful of constraints, we can observe quite simply from where arise the differential phonotactic patterns in English and in Spanish. While this analysis may be slightly idealized, it demonstrates the key idea upon which the current section hinges: cross-linguistic differences in the ranking of *Complex give rise to distinct relative syllabic frequencies in Spanish and English. As such, this paper capitalizes on these distinct rankings of the *Complex constraint while examining natural speech from a Spanish-English corpus. If bilinguals do indeed possess encapsulated phonological systems (in line with MacSwan’s model and the constraint-free approach), the hierarchy of constraints in code-switched speech should respect the hierarchy of constraints of each respective language involved in the code-switch.

3.2 Methods

3.2.1 The corpus

The publicly available Bangor-Miami corpus was consulted (see Deuchar et al. 2014; available at http://bangortalk.org.uk through the Free Software Foundation’s General Public License). This corpus contains transcribed audio files of several hours of recorded speech from Spanish-English bilinguals in the region of Miami, Florida. The current paper focused on the recordings (n = 15) from a 45-year-old female speaker with the pseudonym ‘Maria’, who was known to the researchers as an avid code-switcher and a balanced bilingual. As such, she was given a microphone and invited to record code-switched conversations in her daily life (Deuchar et al. 2014). The corpus contains a total running time of approximately 14 hours of Maria’s speech.

Maria’s recordings were chosen over those of other speakers in the corpora, since her conversations took place in-the-field. In fact, Maria was conversing with friends, family and colleagues (Deuchar et al. 2014), and her recordings are thus very likely to reflect her natural speech. Recent reviews indeed call for both a) the use of habitual code-switching participants; and b) more ecologically valid data sets or collection methods (González-Vilbazo et al. 2013; Beatty-Martínez, Valdés Kroff & Dussias 2018; Blanco-Elorrieta & Pylkkänen 2018; van Hell, Fernandez, Kootstra, Litcofsky & Ting 2018). By using Maria’s spontaneous recordings, these methodological considerations are addressed.

3.2.2 Coding procedures and variables of interest

Based off the audio files and their transcriptions, sentences produced by Maria were coded according to two crucial variables: Context and Language. The variable of Context refers to the type of utterance: code-switch or unilingual. Code-switched segments correspond to streams of speech containing both Spanish and English, as shown in the examples in (9). Unilingual segments correspond to streams of speech occurring in either English (10a) or Spanish (10b). In order to avoid possible inter-sentential switching points, segments were only extracted and coded as unilingual if they were preceded by at least one other single-language sentence in the same language. Occurrences of single other-language items such as (11a–b) were excluded, since they may be borrowed words; attested loanwords were also omitted (see Section 2.1).

The variable of Language simply corresponds to whether each segment was produced in Spanish or in English, regardless of Context. In code-switched sentences, the portions of speech are also coded as belonging to Spanish or English (e.g., from (9b): tienen ese salon grande, pero en, etc. are coded as Spanish and county, so it’s very easy to hang out, etc. as English). The language tag (Spanish or English) is determined by the lexicon of origin of each word (i.e., Lx or Ly in Figure 1). For instance, in (9a), menos is a lexical item in Spanish, but not in English. In the case of cross-linguistic homonyms (e.g., tan in means ‘so’ in Spanish, and ‘shade of brown’ in English), the syntactic context provided a disambiguation for the language tag (e.g., for (9b) to be well-formed, tan must possess the Spanish meaning and is thus tagged as belonging to Spanish). Notice that the phonotactics of the words are not consulted during the coding of the language tags.

Though Spanish and English share a high number of cognates, near-cognates (e.g., resistir/resist, compañía/company, etc.) are more frequent than identical cognates (e.g., natural/natural, cables/cables) (see Schepens, Dijkstra & Grootjen 2012); this was also the case in Maria’s speech. Thus, the language tag of the cognates could be established according to lexical and morphological features of the word. For example, resistir in (9c) contains Spanish verbal inflection, and is thus tagged as Spanish; similarly, compañía possesses the Spanish feminine declension class -a, and is thus tagged as Spanish. With respect to orthographically identical cognates, single phonemes were used to disambiguate the language tag. For instance, natural may be tagged as Spanish if the rhotic is produced as /ɾ/ rather than /ɹ/. Consequently, even in the case of cognates, no reference to the phonotactics of the word is required in order to attribute a language tag to Maria’s segments of speech. Note that proper nouns and other pronounced speech sounds were not analyzed (‘Henry’, ‘Naomi’, ‘uh’, ‘oh’, ‘woo’, etc.), as the language tag is often ambiguous.

(9) Code-switched sentences (Spanish/English)
  a. We may have to put the house shields you know home house shields on those para por lo menos quitar un poquitico porque he wants to at least show the mira cuando no tenia un shield […]. (Maria, 7, 61)
    ‘We may have to put the house shields you know home house shields on those in order to at least remove a little bit because he wants to at least show the… look when he didn’t have a shield…’
  b. In XXX county tienen ese salon grande so it’s very easy to hang out pero en XXX county no es tan comfy como eso. (Maria, 24, 588)
    ‘In XXX county they have that big lounge so it’s very easy to hang out but in XXX county it’s not so comfy as that.’
  c. No lo pude resistir cause they were too cute. (Maria, 40, 153)
    ‘No I can’t resist because they were too cute.’
(10) Unilingual sentences (Spanish/English)
  a. I saw that we have some proposal due on the tenth. Are you going to want me to review it? (Maria, 2, 1–5).
  b. Pedro me dijo. Vamos a dar tragos a todo el mundo. (Maria, 16, 48–49).
    ‘Pedro told me. We’re offering drinks to everybody.’
(11) Excluded other single-language items (Spanish/English)
  a. Mi hermano llego el sabado antes de Christmas. (Maria, 4, 164)
    ‘My brother came the Saturday just before Christmas.’
  b. They are not going to remove it but they wanna be able to say bueno you know it’s no different than whatever your house your street light or something. (Maria, 7, 59)

Once each segment was coded for Context and Language, its corresponding syllables were manually extracted and, based on impressionistic judgements, categorized as one of the following types: CV, CVC, V, VC, CCV, VCC, CCV, CCVC, CVCC, CCVCC, CVCCC, CCVCCC, VCCC, CCCVC and CCCV. This process was accomplished with reference to both the transcription and the corresponding auditory files for Maria’s conversations. In the case of any uncertainty (i.e., degraded sound quality, differences between the transcription and the audio recording), Spanish informants were consulted (see acknowledgements) to identify the syllables. Since vowel quality was of little interest to the current project, syllables with simplex vowels and diphthongs were collapsed into a single syllabic type.

Note that only intra-sentential (and not inter-sentential) code-switches were extracted (see 9), since this is the environment in which phonological convergence is most likely to materialize. In other words, if convergence were to take place in code-switched speech, it is most probable during instances of lengthy switches, when Maria is freely using both of her languages to express her thoughts. If convergence does not appear even in these types of contexts, it serves as robust evidence that Maria is actively alternating between compartmentalized phonological systems.

3.2.3 Potential empirical outcomes

The syllabic structure extracted from Maria’s code-switched speech was compared to that of her unilingual speech. More specifically, the proportions for each syllable type within the Spanish portions of Maria’s code-switched speech were compared to their relative proportions in the Spanish portions of Maria’s unilingual conversations. Similarly, the proportions for each syllable type within the English portions of Maria’s code- switched speech were compared to their relative frequency in the English portions of Maria’s single-language conversations. Since the constraints relative to syllable structure (e.g., *Complex) vary between Spanish and English, Maria’s use of different syllables structures during code-switched speech will give insight into the underlying phonological system(s) that is—or are—employed when she mixes her languages. As such, the possible empirical outcomes from (7) are specified to Maria’s speech in (12).

(12) Potential empirical outcomes (let Spanish be Lx, English be Ly and consonant clusters be CC):
  A. Evidence for the encapsulation of the phonological systems:
    i. %CC in Lx within code-switching = %CC in Lx within unilingual AND
    ii. %CC in Ly within code-switching = %CC in Ly within unilingual
  B. Evidence against the encapsulation of the phonological systems:
    i. %CC in Lx within code-switching > %CC in Lx within unilingual OR
    ii. %CC in Ly within code-switching < %CC in Ly within unilingual

To be clear, outcome A) entails that Maria is capable of abruptly switching between the different OT grammars of her languages, and thus that her phonological systems are encapsulated. On the other hand, outcome B) suggests that Maria does not experience a sudden shift in phonological systems, and that her language faculty may not be composed of compartmentalized phonologies.

Maria’s data may yield outcome B) if she resyllabifies the lexical items in her code-switches or repairs a consonant cluster with the help of faithfulness constraints (see the hypothetical examples 13a–c). Indeed, such cluster reductions are attested in Spanish-influenced dialects of English in the United States, such as Chicano English (Santa Ana & Bayley 2008) and Puerto Rican English (Bayley & Bonnici 2009).4 Alternatively, outcome B) may arise if Maria simply implicitly avoids lexical items that would be problematic for an integrated phonological system (see the hypothetical examples 14a–c). Rather than phonological resyllabification, this second alternative entails that Maria reformulates her message—or at the very least, that she executes lexical substitutions or insertions—in order to avoid computational complexity in the integrated phonological system. In any case, both resolutions involve a difference in the relative syllabic structure between Maria’s unilingual and code-switched speech (12B).

(13) Resyllabification
  a. I drove el coche. ‘I drove the car.’
    = Convergence of ‘drove’ towards Spanish: /ɑ͡ɪ.dɹov / ➔ /ɑ͡ɪd.ɹov/
  b. The libro se ha caído off the table. ‘The book has fallen off the table.’
    = Convergence of ‘libro’ towards English: /ðə.lib.ro/ ➔ /ðə.li.bro/
  c. I sent la carta a mi mom. ‘I sent the letter to my mom.’
    = Convergence of ‘sent’ towards Spanish: /ɑ͡ɪ.sɛnt / ➔ /ɑ͡ɪ.sɛn/
(14) Lexical reformulations
  a. I drove el coche. ‘I drove the car.’
    = Cluster avoidance: I drove el coche. ➔ Conduje (/kon.du.je/) the car.
  b. The libro se ha caído off the table. ‘The book has fallen off the table.’
    = Cluster pursuit: The libro ➔ The libro gris (/ðə.lib.ro.gris/)
  c. I sent la carta a mi mom. ‘I sent the letter to my mom.’
    = Cluster avoidance: I sent ➔ I posted (/pos.tɪd/)

As per MacSwan’s minimalist model of the bilingual language faculty, the analysis is expected to yield outcome A; the relative frequency of syllabic structure used by Maria is not hypothesized to differ significantly from her unilingual speech in each respective language.

3.2.4 Analyses

In order to verify this hypothesis empirically, two separate analyses were realized: generalized linear models and repeated measure ANOVAs. As the frequencies of the syllable types are interdependent for a given analysis, and are skewed for values below 0.5, all proportion values were transformed into logits. This was the dependent variable in both analyses.

Firstly, three generalized linear models were constructed for the data (see Supplementary material for R script). The first (“Baseline”) contained the following independent (categorical) predictors: Onset (zero Cs, one C, two Cs, three Cs) and Coda (zero Cs, one C, two Cs, three Cs). In the second (“LanguageModerator”) and third (“ContextModerator”) models, the additional categorical predictor of, respectively, Language (English or Spanish) and Context (unilingual or code-switched), were introduced. The interaction between this additional predictor and each other initial predictor was also included in the last two models (e.g., Context*Onset, Context*Coda, Context*Onset*Coda). The goal of this analysis was to examine the goodness of fit of each model, in order to assess whether the introduction of Language or Context enable the model to make better predictions about the frequency of each syllable type. The goodness of fit of each model was interpreted via the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike 1987). The AIC gives information about the quality of a given model: the lesser the AIC, the better the fit of the model. AIC is useful, as it penalizes the addition of any new predictor to the model. This prevents more complex models from being a better fit simply by virtue of being more complex. A difference in AIC larger than +/–2 between models is interpreted as a significant difference between the models. If Maria is using compartmentalized phonological systems when she is code-switching, the inclusion of Language should provide the truest model for the data.

The second analysis was used to confirm the results from the AIC analysis. Separate 2 x 2 repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted for each syllable type (i.e., CV, CVC, V, VC… etc.) using the data from the disaggregated audio-files (see Supplementary material for summarized data). This wasn’t accomplished for CCCVCC and CCVCCC syllable types due to lack of data. The ANOVAs included the experimental factors of Context and Language. Note that audio files (n = 15) represent different conversations recorded by Maria (i.e., different times, places and co-interlocutors). This type of analysis allows us to treat the audio file as a factor potentially introducing variability into the results. Furthermore, it tests for differences according to Language and/or Context within individual syllable types, giving us more fine-grained insight into the data. If Maria’s phonological systems are indeed separate, Language should produce significant differences between syllable types, but not Context.

3.3 Results

3.3.1 The data

From the 14 hours of Maria’s recorded conversations, 37 609 syllables were coded, with 88% of syllables belonging to unilingual speech (within unilingual speech: English = 69%; Spanish = 31%) and 12% belonging to intra-sentential code-switching (within code-switches: English = 45%; Spanish = 55%). The number and proportions belonging to each syllable type according to the experimental factors (Context and Language) are detailed in Table 4 and depicted visually in Figure 2. Note that Maria’s syllable proportions largely match those that have been reported previously for unilingual English and Spanish speech (see Table 1).

Figure 2
Figure 2

Proportion of syllables according to Context and Language.

Table 4

Overall summary of coded data, collapsed across audio files.

Unilingual EN CSwitched EN Unilingual SP CSwitched SP
n % n % n % n %
CV 6954 31.20 % 638 29.85 % 6086 56.93 % 1461 59.02 %
CVC 7877 33.72 % 697 36.45 % 1681 17.25 % 402 15.95 %
V 1480 6.07 % 119 6.44 % 839 11.11 % 205 8.15 %
VC 2459 11.33 % 201 10.98 % 796 6.41 % 212 9.37 %
CCV 567 2.51 % 59 2.57 % 549 5.65 % 132 5.42 %
CCCV 5 0.03 % 1 0.02 % 1 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
VCC 684 3.08 % 65 2.57 % 1 0.06 % 1 0.02 %
VCCC 7 0.05 % 1 0.06 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
CCVC 593 2.39 % 46 2.14 % 310 2.57 % 53 1.94 %
CVCC 1856 7.92 % 165 7.20 % 7 0.03 % 2 0.06 %
CCVCC 186 0.86 % 16 0.54 % 0 0.00 % 2 0.06 %
CVCCC 133 0.72 % 20 0.96 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
CCCVC 28 0.10 % 4 0.23 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
CCCVCC 2 0.02 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
CCVCCC 6 0.02 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 % 0 0.00 %
TOTAL 22837 100% 2032 100% 10270 100% 2470 100%

3.3.2 Generalized linear models

The results from all three models are presented in Table 5. As can be seen, the inclusion of the Language predictor (LanguageModerator) produced the best fitted model; its AIC is the smallest in magnitude, and the difference in AIC between LanguageModerator and both other models is larger than 2. As such, even when additional predictors were penalized, Language (Spanish vs. English) improved the predictability of the data. ContextModerator was not better fitted than the baseline model (difference in AIC = 0.5), indicating that Context (monolingual vs code-switched) does not improve the goodness of fit of the model to the data.

Table 5

AIC results from the generalized linear models (null deviance = 95.535, df = 59).

AIC Delta AIC
Baseline 155.8 5.9
LanguageModerator 149.9 0.00
ContextModerator 155.3 5.4

3.3.3 Repeated-measure ANOVAs

The results from the ANOVAs are presented in Table 6. As can be seen, the factor of Language produced significant (or near-significant) differences between syllable proportions for the following syllable types: CVC, V, VC, CCV, CCCV, VCC, CVCC, CCVCC, CVCCC. The factor of Context was only significant for one syllable type (devoid of any consonant clusters): VC. Based on these results, it is clear that Language (i.e., belonging to an English vs. a Spanish portion of speech) is a better predictor of syllable identification than Context (i.e., belonging to a unilingual vs. a code-switched portion of speech).

Table 6

Results from RM-ANOVA. Significant p-values are flagged with asterisk(s) and bolded; p-values trending towards significance are also bolded. In all cases, df = (1,14).

Syll. Type Effect Mean Square F p η2p
CV Context 2.023 3.180 .096 .185
Language 3.603 3.480 .083 .199
CVC Context 1.602 3.212 .095 .187
Language 10.093 21.183 < .001** .602
V Context .048 0.152 .702 .011
Language .962 4.361 .056 .238
VC Context 2.227 8.303 .012* .372
Language 2.734 5.251 .038* .273
CCV Context .008 0.019 .892 .001
Language 4.677 8.631 .011* .381
CCCV Context <.001 0.213 .651 .015
Language .007 4.171 .060 .230
VCC Context .143 2.069 .172 .129
Language 20.491 92.813 < .001** .869
VCCC Context <.001 0.010 .923 <.001
Language .023 2.145 .165 .133
CCVC Context .111 0.501 .491 .035
Language .046 0.259 .619 .018
CVCC Context .005 0.023 .883 .002
Language 57.636 278.786 < .001** .952
CCVCC Context .081 1.712 .212 .109
Language 2.755 35.209 < .001** .715
CVCCC Context .006 0.084 .776 .006
Language 3.775 35.323 < .001** .716
CCCVC Context <.001 0.019 .894 .001
Language .129 3.003 .105 .177

3.4 Discussion

In line with the hypotheses, statistical models indicated that the language of production of each syllable was a significant predictor in determining syllabic frequency in intra-sentential code-switches. Context was neither a significant predictor for syllabic use in Spanish nor in English. As such, even when Maria was code-switching, the Spanish portions of her mixed utterances were Spanish-like, and the English portions of her mixed utterances were English-like (see assumption A in (12)). By contrast, context was neither a significant predictor for syllabic use in Spanish nor in English—Maria was not using an excess of complex onsets and codas in the Spanish portions of her code-switched speech, and she was not limiting these same syllabic structures in the English portions. These main findings demonstrate that Maria was actively and abruptly switching between the encapsulated phonological systems of her language.

Recall that MacSwan suggests that code-switching at the phonological form of language is unavailable, since PF is composed of hierarchical constraints: if the phonological forms were united, in a code-switching context, the order of the constraints would not be upheld. In consequence, when a derivation arrives at Spell-Out, two separate phonologies (PSx and PSy) are compartmentalized before PF. This means that lexical items originating from either lexicon can only be conditioned by Lx or Ly. The results from the current study thoroughly support MacSwan’s model: Maria’s language-specific orderings of *Complex were preserved during code-switching (i.e., the syllabic structure in her code-switched speech matched the syllabic structure of respective monolingual speech). Consequently, the current data also fail to support models which maintain that bilinguals possess fully integrated grammars (see López 2020); Maria displayed no convergence between her phonological systems. The interface of phonology is clearly dichotomized in the bilingual language faculty.

How can independent phonological systems be analyzed within OT? Preliminary considerations tend to support the notion of co-phonologies. The formalism of co-phonology has mostly been applied to morphologically conditioned phonological systems. In a co-phonology (e.g., Inkelas 1998; Inkelas & Zoll 2007), different morphological components (within a single language) are treated with different grammars (e.g., one for suffixes, one for roots). Co-phonologies have previously been implemented for words from different lexical classes (Inkela & Zoll 2007). From this line of thinking, it is not a huge leap to suppose that co-phonologies can be applied to different lexical sets, as in code-switching.

A co-phonology seems to correspond to what was observed in Maria’s code-switched speech: Maria abruptly alternated from one phonological system to the next when she mixed Spanish and English. In a code-switching co-phonology, each language involved in the utterance would possess its own compartmentalized phonological system. That is, phonological systems would map to a single language involved in the switch. Spoken works would thus be parsed by the appropriate system, depending on the language from which they originate.

3.5 Section summary

Up to this point, the minimalist model of the bilingual language faculty (MacSwan 2000) has been supported. Utilizing the phonotactic differences between Spanish and English (i.e., regarding the *Complex constraint), the speech of a Spanish-English code-switcher was analyzed in order to determine whether she abruptly alternated between her phonological systems during code-switching. The analysis revealed differences in syllabic proportion between languages (Spanish vs. English), but not between contexts (unilingual vs. code-switched). These results reinforce the idea, in line with MacSwan’s model of bilingual language faculty, that A) distinct phonological systems are accessed during code-switching and B) that these systems correspond to those of each monolingual language involved.

We can now answer the first half of the primary research question: the bilingual language faculty is composed of two compartmentalized phonological systems. Just like MacSwan originally conjectured, “language switching is phonological switching” (MacSwan & Colina 2014: 188). In the next section, I will answer the second part of the research question. That is, I will review theoretical evidence that suggests that code-switching is composed of a single syntactic grammar. This evidence will once again be supported by in-the-field instances of Maria’s code-switched speech.

4 Syntax: Single and ready to mingle

4.1 Background

While the independence of the phonological systems is supported, this does not reflect the entire conceptualization of the bilingual language faculty. Recall that MacSwan’s model also ascertains that bilinguals (just like Chomsky’s monolingual) possess a computational system (CHL) which combines items into a single syntactic derivation. That is, once items are chosen from the lexicon(s), the array is Spelled-Out in one fell swoop (or, potentially, in a series of phases; see López et al. 2017); structures are built when items are derivationally combined via the operation of Merge (MacSwan 2000).

Up to this point, I have taken the singularity of CHL for granted. Nonetheless, another option is possible when it comes to the bilingual language faculty: individuals who speak more than one language may possess globally compartmentalized systems for each language, as in Figure 3 (right panel). Luis López (2017 and onward) coins this framework as separationism or the common-sense framework, since it is reflective of the way humans intuitively think about language (e.g., “I speak three languages, but my mom only speaks two.”). As MacSwan’s model also involves some degree of architectural separation (i.e., at the level of phonology), I will refer to the common-sense framework as global separationism. Global separationism entails that bilinguals’ morphosyntactic grammars5 are just as independent as their phonological systems.

Figure 3
Figure 3

Left: Simplified version of MacSwan’s (2000) model of the bilingual language faculty. Right: The bilingual language faculty from a globally separatist perspective (adapted from López 2020: 17).

Crucially, global separationism and MacSwan’s model make different assumptions with regard to multi-language structures. If global separationism is correct, the bilingual language faculty would only be able to Merge items from a single lexicon, since the CHL is language-dependent, or language-encapsulated. Contrastively, MacSwan’s minimalist model of the bilingual language faculty allows for the combination of items originating from multiple languages.

You may think that global separationism is immediately falsified, since bilinguals evidently do combine items from more than one lexicon—that is the very core of intra-sentential code-switching, after all. However, a sceptic might argue that the mere existence of code-switching does not negate global separationism, since bilinguals may simply be building partial syntactic structures within their compartmentalized computational systems, before pronouncing these structures in juxtaposition. In other words, some may argue that code-switching has the outward appearance of a combined syntactic system, but that it is actually composed of independent structures being outputted side-by-side. This would entail that code-switching is a surface phenomenon that exists in performance but not in competence.

How can we verify whether this is the case? It seems that the solution lies in the examination of syntactic dependencies (i.e., c-command, grammatical case, complement relations), which presupposes the singularity of CHL. The theoretical outcomes are the following.

(15) Theoretical outcomes
  A. If syntactic dependencies are found between items of Ly and Lx, code-switching can be represented by Gx^y.
  B. If items from Ly and Lx do not interact syntactically, code-switching cannot be represented by Gx^y.

The diverse syntactic code-switching literature, as well as Maria’s speech from the Bangor-Miami corpus, provide us with ample opportunities to verify these assumptions. Much of the argumentations provided in this section are inspired by Luis López’ generativist body of work, most notably his recent book (2020). Recall that, unlike MacSwan, López believes that the bilingual language faculty is combined at every interface, including PF. While the syllabic data from Section 3 markedly refutes the integration of a bilingual’s phonological systems, this does not mean that López’ entire model is unsupported. In fact, MacSwan and López appear to agree in their assumption that bilinguals derive code-switches from a unique syntactic subset. Maria’s naturalistic data provides more evidence for this conjecture.

Given that much of the evidence provided in the subsequent sections was also presented by López (2020), novel data (i.e., Maria’s data from the Bangor-Miami corpus; syntactic judgements obtained by the author) are identified as such.

4.2 Negative polarity items

According to Hoeksema (2000: 115), negative polarity items (NPIs) are linguistic units that “give rise to minimal pairs of affirmative and negative sentences”. Crucially, in order to produce grammatical sentences, NPIs must be c-commanded (see Chomsky 2014) by negative elements, like not (Giannakidou 2011). Thus, the difference between the sentences in (16) is that any is licensed (or triggered) in (16a) but not in (16b).

(16) a.   I do not have any more candy.
  b. *I have any more candy.

Past data (and novel judgements)6 indicate that sentences containing opposite-language NPIs and negation triggers are acceptable, as indicated in (17–19). In each case, the NPI is bolded and the license is underlined. Under a globally separatist point of view, such sentences should be ill-formed, since syntactic relations like c-command can only take place within the language-compartmentalized CHL. On the other hand, MacSwan’s model predicts that such constructions should be entirely acceptable, since lexical arrays are built into a single syntactic structure and can thus Command one another.

    1. (17)
    1. Spanish / Basque (Vergara & Lopéz 2017 : 270)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Ez
    2. neg
    1. zen
    2. aux
    1. nadie
    2. anyone
    1. etorri
    2. come
    1. etxera.
    2. home
    1. ‘No one came home.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. No
    2. neg
    1. vino
    2. come
    1. inor
    2. anyone
    1. a casa.
    2. home
    1. ‘Nobody came home.’
    1. (18)
    1. English / Canadian French (novel data; author’s judgements)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Y’a
    2. there is
    1. pas de
    2. no
    1. snow
    2. snow
    1. anymore.
    2. anymore
    1. / *
    2.  
    1. Y’a
    2. there is
    1. du
    2.  
    1. snow
    2. snow
    1. anymore.
    2. anymore
    1. ‘There’s no snow anymore.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Jean
    2. Jean
    1. lave
    2. washes
    1. jamais
    2. never
    1. ever
    2. ever
    1. sa
    2. his
    1. van.
    2. van
    1. / *
    2.  
    1. Jean
    2. Jean
    1. lave
    2. washes
    1. ever
    2. ever
    1. sa
    2. his
    1. van.
    2. van
    1. ‘Jean doesn’t ever wash his van.’
    1. (19)
    1. English / Lebanese Arabic (novel data; personal communications)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. He’s
    2. he is
    1. not
    2. not
    1. better
    2. better
    1. than
    2. than
    1. 7adan.
    2. anyone
    1. ‘He’s not better than anyone.’

In each of these cases, a syntactic dependency is established between an NPI and a trigger that originate from distinct lexical sets. That is, a negative particle from Lx licences an NPI from Ly. Thus, the singularity of the syntactic component is supported: the examples above clearly demonstrate that code-switching is not simply the juxtaposition of two partial syntactic structures constructed in language-dependent CHL. In actuality, the lexical items from the code-switched language pairs are united into a combined array, from which the unique CHL can build a single syntactic construction.

4.3 Grammatical case

Grammatical case is associated to the syntactic or grammatical function of an item within a given sentence. Many languages possess complex case systems, wherein grammatical case is indicated by an inflectional morpheme; the sentence becomes unacceptable in the absence of case marking or when the wrong case is assigned. For instance, in (20b) bedny- ‘poor’ is assigned the genitive case instead of the dative case (as in 20a), and the resulting sentence is ill-formed.

    1. (20)
    1. Russian (Babby 1987)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1.   Bogatye
    2.   the-rich.nom
    1. nijogda
    2. never
    1. ne
    2. neg
    1. zavidujut
    2. envy
    1. bednym.
    2. the-poor-dat
    1.    ‘The rich never envy the poor.’
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. *Bogatye
    2.   the-rich-nom
    1. nikogda
    2. never
    1. ne
    2. neg
    1. zavidujut
    2. envy
    1. bednyx
    2. the-poor-gen

If case assignment occurs between a verb from Lx and an item from Ly, we can say that there is a syntactic dependency between these items, and that they co-exist within the same CHL. Past evidence suggests that these syntactic dependencies do indeed take place in code-switching (21–24). For instance, in the sluicing constructions in (21), the verb ‘to threaten’ assigns the dative case in German (21a), but the accusative case in Spanish (21b). However, in the code-switch, the German remnant ‘who’ must possess the accusative case (21c). This judgement was confirmed by an acceptability task (González-Vilbazo & Ramos 2011). López (2020) also gives evidence that these types of constructions occur (23–24).

    1. (21)
    1. German / Spanish (González-Vilbazo & Ramos 2018)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Juan
    2. Juan
    1. hat
    2. has
    1. jemandem
    2. someone.dat
    1. gedroht
    2. threatened
    1. aber
    2. but
    1. ich
    2. I
    1. weiß
    2. know
    1. nicht
    2. not
    1. {*wer/ *wen/wem.
    2. who.nom/acc/dat
    1.  
    1. b.
    1. Juan
    2. Juan
    1. amenazó
    2. threatened
    1. a
    2.  
    1. alguien
    2. someone.acc
    1. pero
    2. but
    1. no
    2. not
    1. know
    1. {*quién / a quién}.
    2. who.nom / acc
    1.  
    1. c.
    1. Juan
    2. Juan
    1. amenazó
    2. threatened
    1. a
    2.  
    1. alguien
    2. someone.acc
    1. aber
    2. but
    1. ich
    2. I
    1. weiß
    2. know
    1. nicht
    2. not
    1. {wen / *wem}.
    2. who.acc / dat
    1. ‘Juan threatened someone, but I don’t know who.
    1. (22)
    1. Basque / Spanish (González-Vilbazo & Ramos 2018)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Algún
    2. some
    1. ancian-o
    2. old
    1. maestr-o-k
    2. teacher-.erg
    1. fabrika-n
    2. factory-loc
    1. bai
    2. indeed
    1. barre
    2. laugh
    1. egiten
    2. do
    1. dut.
    2. aux
    1. ‘Some old teacher(m/f) laughs in the factory.’
    1. (23)
    1. German / Turkish (Lopéz 2020)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Neu-e
    2. new
    1. Film-i
    2. film-acc
    1. sehen
    2. see
    1. yapiyor.
    2. do
    1. ‘Seeing a new film.’
    1. (24)
    1. Russian / Kazakh (Lopéz 2020; originally from Auer & Muhamedova 2005)
    1.  
    1. a.
    1. Anau
    2. this
    1. star-y
    2. old-nom.acc
    1. ploshod-
    2. square-acc
    1. ne-ler-di
    2. thing-acc
    1. zönder-di.
    2. renovate
    1. ‘There the old square and so were were already renovated.’

Altogether, these data illustrate that grammatical case assignment can take place between items from different languages. As case assignment entails syntactic dependency (assumption A), it is clear that the intra-sentential code-switches shown above were derived from a singular syntactic construction. The evidence above therefore supports the singularity of the syntactic component, in line with MacSwan’s formulation of the bilingual language faculty.

4.4 Mixed selection

López (e.g. 2020) originally introduced the ‘Mixed Selection’ problem by examining code-switching between light verbs and infinitives. To generalize this concept, I assume that Mixed Selection has occurred when an item from Lx selects one from Ly as its complement, or vice versa.

4.4.1 Determiner phrases

The general consensus in syntax is that constituents such as that flower, la flor are headed by the determiner (see Bernstein 1991). In Spanish and English, the determiner-head requires a nominal complement. For instance, the sentence in (25) becomes unacceptable if the noun phrase within the scope of the DP is empty.

(25) a. Ben went to the store to buy apples. / *Ben went to the to buy apples.
  b. Ben fue a la tienda a comprar manzanas. / *Ben fue a la a comprar manzanas.

Thus, if a determiner from Lx selects a nominal complement from Ly, we can assume that Mixed Selection has occurred. Under global separationism, the derivation containing the DP should crash within the language specific CHL, since the complement position would be empty. However, if we assume that code-switching is built from a single syntactic construction, Mixed Selection involving determiners and nouns is entirely unproblematic.

Mixed determiner phrases were copious in Maria’s spontaneous speech (26). There also exist innumerable examples in the code-switching literature (Ewing 1984; Köppe & Meisel 1995; MacSwan 2000; Liceras Fernández Fuertes, Perales, Pérez-Tattam & Spradlin 2008; Myers-Scotton & Jake 2009; Gonzalez-Vilbazo & López 2011; Burkholder 2018; López 2020; etc.). Thus, Mixed Selection between determiners and nouns supports MacSwan’s minimalist model of the bilingual language faculty.

(26) Maria’s English / Spanish (Bangor Miami Corpus) (novel data)
  a. I mean you can see the big las pieces of property (Maria, 1, 68)
    ‘I mean you can see the big, the big pieces of property…’
  b. Es el north side so it doesn’t get any sun. (Maria, 1, 119–120)
    ‘It’s the north side so it doesn’t get any sun.’
  c. Me dice no es que tienen el heating thing. (Maria, 1, 289–290)
    ‘She told me no, it’s that they had the heating thing.’
  d. Chica te hace falta un new calendar. (Maria, 2, 36)
    ‘Girl, you need a new calendar.’
  e. Dejame ver ya guardaste tu pink shirt. (Maria, 16, 189)
    ‘Let me see, did you already put away your pink shirt?’
  f. Cuándo es el swearing in? (Maria, 18, 830)
    ‘When is the swearing in (ceremony)?’

4.4.2 Verbal complements

Some transitive and ditransitive verbs require complements. For instance, the sentence in (27), which contains the transitive verb ‘to buy’/‘comprar’, is rendered unacceptable if the object DP is absent. In this case, one would say that the verbal head ‘selects’ (or Merges with) its complement, ‘a pair of socks’/‘un par de calcetines’.

(27) a.   Mona bought a pair of socks. / Mona compró un par de calcetines.
  b. *Mona bought. / *Mona compró.

If an obligatorily transitive verb from Lx selects a complement from Ly, this means that Mixed Selection has occurred. There are countless examples of this type of Mixed Selection in Maria’s naturally produced speech (28). It is also abundant in the code-switching literature (Pfaff 1979; Ewing 1984; Azuma 1993; Mahootian & Santorini 1996; Gonzalez-Vilbazo & López 2011). The presence of such constructions provides solid evidence of a united syntactic component, in line with MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty. If the CHL were language-dependent, the partial structure involving the verb would crash before Spell-Out in the absence of its complement.

(28) Maria’s English / Spanish (Bangor Miami Corpus) (novel data)
  a. Si es four-wheel drive? (Maria, 1, 33)
    ‘If it’s four-wheel drive?’
  b. Mi carro tiene heated seats. (Maria, 1, 283)
    ‘My car has heated seats.’
  c. In fact, I was watching las noticias y había una mujer que salio con identical triplets. (Maria, 20, 245–246)
    ‘In fact I was watching the news and there was a woman on the news who has had identical triplets.’
  d. Pero ellos no querían multiple kids. (Maria, 20, 249)
    ‘But they didn’t want multiple kids.’
  e. Comiendo two bowls of cereal todas las noches… (Maria, 27, 177)
    ‘Eating two bowls of cereal every night.’
  f. No creo que haya orange soda mama. (Maria, 40, 236)
    ‘I don’t think there’s orange soda honey.’

4.5 Section summary

In the current section, the second half of MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty was supported: code-switching is in fact governed by a singular morphosyntactic grammar. The evidence from the literature and Maria’s spontaneous speech shows that syntactic dependencies do indeed take place between different-language items. This is not predicted by global separationism, which presupposes globally encapsulated systems for each language known by a bilingual or multilingual.

We can now answer the second half of the primary research question. Contra the “common-sense” hypothesis, the bilingual language faculty is composed of a unique computational system, wherein syntactic constructions are built.

5 Concluding remarks

The current paper has examined the architecture of the bilingual language faculty by reviewing code-switching data. As MacSwan originally conjectured, bilingual cognition is composed of a single computational system, which can build multi-language syntactic constructions, and two phonological systems. MacSwan’s model was supported using both empirical (corpus-based) data and theoretical frameworks. This means that code-switching, essentially, is ‘just phonological switching”. Interestingly, neuroimaging studies have shown that language switching does indeed engage brain regions associated to phonological processing (Price, Green & von Studnitz 1999).

This paper is not only relevant for formal bilingualism research; it may also allow researchers to contextualize past experimental studies. For instance, past research has demonstrated that cognates (i.e., orthographically and/or phonologically similar words cross-linguistically, such as table in French and English) may act as catalysts for code-switching (e.g., Brown 2015). That is, the alternation between languages (code-switching) is most likely to occur after a cognate has been produced. This finding also appears to be predicted by the architecture of the bilingual language faculty. If the assembling process is cognitively costly, then this effort may be mitigated by the presence of a cognate, since such words possess similar entries in each of the lexicons. Bilinguals may thus utilize this temporary dip in the required cognitive effort to juxtapose items from their languages. Future research should examine this interesting pattern more closely.

Before ending this paper, one last peripheral point can be made about MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty. Recall that, in the pursuit of computational efficiency, MacSwan theorizes that bilinguals possess separate lexicons for each language that they speak (for alternative perspectives see work by Artemis Alexiadou, Kay González-Vilbazo, Luis López and Sara Stefanich). This is not the prevalent view, particularly in psycholinguistics, since a wealth of data indicates that bilinguals possess cooperative or (partially) overlapping lexicons (e.g., Beauvillain & Grainger 1987; Hartsuiker et al. 2004; Sabourin et al. 2014). While this issue is beyond the scope of the present paper, it does not appear that the architecture of the lexicons is particularly pivotal to MacSwan’s model. Whether or not the lexicons are overlapped, the reality remains that bilinguals possess one CHL and two phonological systems. It is not immediately apparent how the presence of an integrated lexicon would challenge these assumptions.

Altogether, in bolstering MacSwan’s model of the bilingual language faculty, this paper supports the constraint-free approach for code-switching, as it achieved by the minimalist program. That is, code-switching is uniquely governed by the languages that compose it, and not a third grammar. Future studies should examine switching using this minimalist tradition.

Supplementary files

The R script for the generalized linear models and the summary of the audio-file disaggregated data are available in the supplementary materials https://doi.org/10.16995/glossa.5800.s1. Full coded data sets are also available upon request.

Abbreviations

Accusative (acc), Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), Auxiliary (aux), Computational system of human language (Chl), Consonant cluster (CC), Dative (dat), Determiner phrase (DP), Ergative (erg), Genitive (gen), Locative (loc), Negation (neg), Negative polarity items (NPI), Nominative (nom), Optimality Theory (OT), Phonological form interface condition (PFIC); Phonological system (PS), Voice onset time (VOT)

Notes

  1. Strikingly, this schematization also appears to stand for bimodal bilinguals. Instead of simply spelling-out a derivation to a given phonological system, bimodal bilinguals may spell-out the structure to the phonology of distinct articulators: speech or sign (see Lillo-Martin, Müeller de Quadros & Chen Pichler 2016). [^]
  2. Delattre and Olsen obtained these syllabic frequencies by examining 2000 syllables in texts from contemporaneous authors (e.g., short stories, plays). [^]
  3. Villanueva-Reyes (2013) originally separated syllables according to whether they contained a simplex vowel or a diphthong. Since vowel quality is of no interest to the current paper, diphtongues and simple vowels were collapsed. It is not specified whether these relative syllabic frequencies were obtained from speech or text samples. [^]
  4. Miami Latino English (colloquially referred to as the “Miami-Dade accent”) appears to present its own dialectal attributes. In particular, linguistic studies have observed differences in the prosody (e.g., Alvord 2006; Carter, López Valdez & Sims 2020), the realization of the lateral /l/ (e.g., Rogers & Alvord 2019) and the realization of low vowels (e.g., Carter, López Valdez & Sims 2020) in Miami English. To my knowledge, research up to this point has not yet examined the phenomenon of cluster reduction in this dialect. [^]
  5. When alluding to the morphosyntactic system or grammar, I refer to this component in a purely minimalist sense. That is, I examine morphemes that are essentially realizations of syntactic features (e.g., case, person, gender, number, etc.). This should not be confused with morphological traits privy to integration, which has often been linked to a change in the phonological system (e.g., MacSwan 2000). [^]
  6. Code-switched NPIs were not observed in Maria’s speech. This does not mean that such constructions are ill-formed in Spanish-English, only that they were not produced by Maria within the corpus (the ‘induction problem’: MacSwan & McAlister 2010). [^]

Acknowledgements

The author would like to thank Flavio Barinas, Mariana Cortés Kandler and Belén López Assef for providing valuable insights in regard to the Spanish-English data. Many thanks to Dr. Kevin McMullin, Dr. Dennis Ott, and Dr. Marc Brunelle for providing thoughtful comments that helped improve this paper.

Competing Interests

The author has no competing interests to declare.

References

Adamou, Evangelina & Shen, Xingjia Rachel. 2019. There are no language switching costs when codeswitching is frequent. International Journal of Bilingualism 23(1). 53–70. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006917709094

Akaike, Hirotugu. 1987. Factor Analysis and AIC. In Parzen, Emanuel & Tanabe, Kunio & Kitagawa, Genshiro (eds.), Selected Papers of Hirotugu Akaike (Springer Series in Statistics), 371–386. Springer, New York, NY. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1694-0_29

Albright, Adam. 2008. Gradient phonological acceptability as a grammatical effect. MIT. Retrieved from http://www.mit.edu/~albright/papers/Albright-GrammaticalGradience.pdf on 11/01/2020.

Alexiadou, Artemis & Lohndal, Terje. 2018. Units of language mixing: A cross-linguistic perspective. Frontiers in Psychology 9. 1719. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01719

Alexiadou, Artemis & Lohndal, Terje & Åfarli, Tor A. & Grimstad, Maren B. 2015. Language mixing: A distributed morphology approach. In Bui, Thuy & Özyildiz, Deniz (eds.), Proceedings of NELS, 45, 25–38. Amherst, MA: GSLA.

Alvord, Scott M. 2006. Spanish Intonation in Contact: The Case of Miami Cuban Bilinguals. Doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.

Anttila, Arto. 2008. Gradient phonotactics and the Complexity Hypothesis. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 26(4). 695–729. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-008-9052-2

Antoniou, Mark & Best, Catherine T. & Tyler, Michael D. & Kroos, Christian. 2011. Inter-language interference in VOT production by L2-dominant bilinguals: Asymmetries in phonetic code-switching. Journal of Phonetics 39(4). 558–570. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.03.001

Auer, Peter & Muhamedova, Raihan. 2005. ‘Embedded language’ and ‘matrix language’ in insertional language mixing: Some problematic cases. Rivista di linguistica 17(1). 35–54.

Azuma, Shoji. 1993. The frame-content hypothesis in speech production: Evidence from intrasentential code switching. Linguistics 31(6). 1071–1094. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1993.31.6.1071

Babby, Leonard H. 1987. Case, prequantifiers, and discontinuous agreement in Russian. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 5(1). 91–138. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/BF00161869

Bailey, Todd M. & Hahn, Ulrike. 2001. Determinants of wordlikeness: Phonotactics or lexical neighborhoods? Journal of Memory and Language 44(4). 568–591. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2756

Balukas, Colleen & Koops, Christian. 2015. Spanish-English bilingual voice onset time in spontaneous code switching. International Journal of Bilingualism 19(4). 423–443. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006913516035

Barlow, Jessica A. 2003. Asymmetries in the acquisition of consonant clusters in Spanish. The Canadian Journal of Linguistics/La revue canadienne de linguistique 48(2). 179–210. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/cjl.2004.0024

Bayley, Robert & Bonnici, Lisa M. 2009. Recent Research on Latinos in the USA and Canada, Part 1: Language Maintenance and Shift and English Varieties 1. Language and Linguistics Compass 3(5), 1300–1313. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2009.00159.x

Beatty-Martínez, Anne L. & Valdés Kroff, Jorge R. & Dussias, Paola E. 2018. From the field to the lab: A converging methods approach to the study of codeswitching. Languages 3(2). 19. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3390/languages3020019

Beauvillain, Cécile & Grainger, Jonathan. 1987. Accessing interlexical homographs: Some limitations of a language-selective access. Journal of Memory and Language 26(6), 658–672. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)90108-2

Belazi, Hedi M. & Rubin, Edward J. & Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline. 1994. Code switching and X-bar theory: The functional head constraint. Linguistic Inquiry 25(2). 221–237. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4178859

Bell, Alan & Brenier, Jason M. & Gregory, Michelle & Girand, Cynthia & Jurafsky, Dan. 2009. Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language 60(1). 92–111. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003

Bernstein, Judy. 1991. The DP Hypothesis: Identifying Clausal Properties in the Nominal Domain. In Baltin, Mark & Collins, Chris (eds.), The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory, 536–561. London Blackwell. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756416.ch17

Besset, Ryan M. 2017. Exploring the Phonological Integration of Lone Other-Language Nouns in the Spanish of Southern Arizona. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 23(2). 5. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol23/iss2/5/

Blanco-Elorrieta, Esti & Pylkkänen, Liina. 2018. Ecological Validity in Bilingualism Research and the Bilingual Advantage. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22(12). 1117–1126. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.001

Bromberger, Sylvain & Halle, Morris. 1989. Why phonology is different. Linguistic Inquiry 20(1). 51–70. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4178613

Brown, Esther L. 2015. The role of discourse context frequency in phonological variation: A usage-based approach to bilingual speech production. International Journal of Bilingualism 19(4). 387–406. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006913516042

Bullock, Barbara & Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline & Davis, Kristopher Allen & Botero, Christopher G. 2004. Phonetic convergence in bilingual Puerto Rican Spanish. In Schmeiser, Benjamin & Chand, Vineeta & Kelleher, Ann & Rodríguez, Angelo (eds.), Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics 23, 113–125. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

Bullock, Barbara E. & Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline & González, Verónica & Dalola, Amanda. 2006. Language dominance and performance outcomes in bilingual pronunciation. In O’Brien, Mary Grantham & Shea, Christine & Archibald, John (eds.), Proceedings of the 8th Generative Approaches to Second Language Acquisition Conference, 9–16. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla.

Burkholder, Michèle. 2018. Language Mixing in the Nominal Phrase: Implications of a Distributed Morphology Perspective. Languages 3(2). 10. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3390/languages3020010

Carter, Phillip M. & Valdez, Lydda López & Sims, Nandi. 2020. New Dialect Formation Through Language Contact: Vocalic and Prosodic Developments in Miami English. American Speech 95(2). 119–148. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1215/00031283-7726313

Chomsky, Noam. 1995/2014. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262527347.001.0001

Chomsky, Noam. 2002. On Nature and Language. Cambridge University Press.

Chomsky, Noam & Gallego, Ángel J. & Ott, Dennis. 2019. Generative grammar and the faculty of language: Insights, questions, and challenges. Catalan Journal of Linguistics. 229–261. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.5565/rev/catjl.288

Coleman, John & Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1997. Stochastic phonological grammars and acceptability. In Coleman, John (ed.), Third Meeting of the ACL Special Interest Group in Computational Phonology: Proceedings of the Workshop, 49–56.

Delattre, Pierre & Olsen, Carroll. 1969. Syllabic features and phonic impression in English, German, French and Spanish. Lingua 22. 160–175. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(69)90051-5

Deuchar, Margaret & Davies, Peredur & Herring, Jon Russell & Couto, M. Carmen Parafita & Carter, Diana. 2014. Building bilingual corpora. In Thomas, Enlli Môn & Mennen, Ineke (eds.), Advances in the Study of Bilingualism, 93–110. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.21832/9781783091713-008

Di Sciullo, Anne-Marie & Muysken, Pieter & Singh, Rajendra. 1986. Government and code-mixing. Journal of Linguistics 22(1). 1–24. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4175815. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700010537

Ewing, Amy. 1984. Polish-English code-switching: A clue to constituent structure and processing mechanism. In Drogo, Joseph & Mishra, Veena & Testen, David (eds.), CLS 20: Papers from the Twentieth Regional Meeting, 53–61. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

Friederici, Angela D. & Wessels, Jeanine M. 1993. Phonotactic knowledge of word boundaries and its use in infant speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics 54(3). 287–295. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3758/BF03205263

Frisch, Stefan A. & Large, Nathan R. & Pisoni, David B. 2000. Perception of wordlikeness: Effects of segment probability and length on the processing of nonwords. Journal of Memory and Language 42(4). 481–496. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2692

Giancaspro, David. 2015. Code-switching at the auxiliary-VP boundary: A comparison of heritage speakers and L2 learners. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 5(3). 379–407. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.5.3.04gia

Giannakidou, Anastasia. 2011. Negative and positive polarity items: Variation, licensing, and compositionality. In Maienborn, Claudia & von Heusinger, Klaus & Portner, Paul (eds.), Semantics: An international handbook of natural language meaning, 1660–1712. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

González-Vilbazo, Kay & Bartlett, Laura & Downey, Sarah & Ebert, Shane & Heil, Jeanne & Hoot, Bradley & Koronkiewicz, Bryan & Ramos, Sergio. 2013. Methodological considerations in code-switching research. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 6(1), 119–138. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/shll-2013-1143

González-Vilbazo, Kay & López, Luis. 2012. Little v and parametric variation. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 30(1). 33–77. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-011-9141-5

González-Vilbazo, Kay & López, Luis. 2011. Some properties of light verbs in code-switching. Lingua 121(5). 832–850. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.11.011

González-Vilbazo, Kay & Ramos, Sergio. 2011. A morphosyntactic condition on sluicing: Evidence from Spanish/German code-switching. University of Illinois at Chicago.

González-Vilbazo, Kay & Ramos, Sergio. 2018. Codeswitching. In van Craenenbroeck, Jeroen & Temmerman, Tanja (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Ellipsis, 458–78. Oxford: Oxford University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198712398.013.21

Gosselin, Leah & Sabourin, Laura. 2021. Lexical-semantic processing costs are not inherent to intra-sentential code-switching: The role of switching habits. Neuropsychologia 159. 107922. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107922

Grosjean, François. 1989. Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language 36(1). 3–15. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5

Grosjean, François. 1995. A psycholinguistic approach to code-switching: The recognition of guest words by bilinguals. In Milroy, Lesley & Muysken, Pieter (eds.), One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching, 259–275. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620867.012

Grosjean, François & Miller, Joanne L. 1994. Going in and out of languages: An example of bilingual flexibility. Psychological Science 5(4). 201–206. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00501.x

Hammond, Michael. 2004. Gradience, phonotactics, and the lexicon in English phonology. International Journal of English Studies 4(2). 1–24. https://revistas.um.es/ijes/article/view/47961

Hartsuiker, Robert J. & Pickering, Martin J. & Veltkamp, Eline. 2004. Is syntax separate or shared between languages? Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in Spanish-English bilinguals. Psychological Science 15(6). 409–414. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00693.x

Heredia, Roberto R. & Altarriba, Jeanette. 2001. Bilingual language mixing: Why do bilinguals code switch? Current Directions in Psychological Science 10(5). 164–168. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00140

Hoeksema, Jack. 2000. Negative polarity items: Triggering, scope, and c-command. In Horn, Laurence R. & Kato, Yasuhiko (eds.), Negation and Polarity: Syntactic and semantic perspectives, 115–146. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Inkelas, Sharon. 1998. The theoretical status of morphologically conditioned phonology: A case study from dominance. In Booij, Geert & van Marle, Jaap (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1997, 121–155. Amsterdam: Springer, Dordrecht. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-4998-3_5

Inkelas, Sharon & Zoll, Cheryl. 2007. Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45(1). 133–171. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/LING.2007.004

Joshi, Aravind. 1982. Processing of sentences with intrasentential code switching. In Hajičová, Eva (eds.), Coling 1982: Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on Computational Linguistics, 190–205. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511597855.006

Jusczyk, Peter W. & Friederici, Angela D. & Wessels, Jeanine M. & Svenkerud, Vigdis Y. & Jusczyk, Ann Marie. 1993. Infants′ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language 32(3). 402–420. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1022

Jusczyk, Peter W. & Luce, Paul A. & Charles-Luce, Jan. 1994. Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language 33(5). 630–645. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1030

Koban, Didem. 2013. Intra-sentential and inter-sentential code-switching in Turkish-English bilinguals in New York City, US. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences 70. 1174–1179. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.01.173

Köppe, Regina & Meisel, Jürgen M. 1995. Code-switching in bilingual first language acquisition. In Milroy, Lesley & Muysken, Pieter (eds.), One speaker, two languages: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on code-switching, 276–301. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620867.013

Liceras, Juana M. & Fuertes, Raquel Fernández & Perales, Susana & Pérez-Tattam, Rocío & Spradlin, Kenton Todd. 2008. Gender and gender agreement in bilingual native and non-native grammars: A view from child and adult functional-lexical mixings. Lingua 118(6). 827–851. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.05.006

Lillo-Martin, Diane & de Quadros, Ronice Müeller & Pichler, Deborah Chen. 2016. The development of bimodal bilingualism: Implications for linguistic theory. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 6(6). 719–755. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.6.6.01lil

López, Luis. 2017. Bilingualism and interfaces. Transcript of the talk at Universidad Autónoma de Madrid. Retrieved online at https://www.academia.edu/35195668/Bilingualism_and_interfaces_talk_Madrid_11_17_2017 on 10/01/2020.

López, Luis. 2020. Bilingual grammar: Toward an integrated model. Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/9781108756181

López, Luis & Alexiadou, Artemis & Veenstra, Tonjes. 2017. Code-switching by phase. Languages 2(3). 9. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3390/languages2030009

López, Verónica González. 2012. Spanish and English word-initial voiceless stop production in code-switched vs. monolingual structures. Second Language Research 28(2). 243–263. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/0267658312439821

MacSwan, Jeff. 2000. The architecture of the bilingual language faculty: Evidence from intrasentential code-switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 3(1). 37–54. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728900000122

MacSwan, Jeff. 2014. Programs and proposals in codeswitching research: Unconstraining theories of bilingual language mixing. In MacSwan, Jeff (ed.), Grammatical Theory and Bilingual Codeswitching, 1–33. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262027892.003.0001

MacSwan, Jeff. 2016. Codeswitching in adulthood. In Nicoladis, Elena & Montanari, Simona (eds.), Lifespan Perspectives on Bilingualism, 183–200. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association & Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1037/14939-011

MacSwan, Jeff & Colina, Sonia. 2014. Some consequences of language design: Codeswitching and the PF interface. In MacSwan, Jeff (ed.), Grammatical Theory and Bilingual Codeswitching, 185–200. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262027892.001.0001

MacSwan, Jeff & McAlister, Kara T. 2010. Naturalistic and elicited data in grammatical studies of codeswitching. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 3(2). 521–32. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/shll-2010-1085

Mahootian, Shahrzad. 1993. A null theory of code switching. Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University.

Mahootian, Shahrzad & Santorini, Beatrice. 1996. Code switching and the complement/adjunct distinction. Linguistic Inquiry 27(3). 464–479. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4178946

Misker, Jan & Anderson, John R. 2003. Combining optimality theory and a cognitive architecture. In Detje, Frank & Dörner, Dietrich & Schaub, Harald (eds.), The logic of cognitive systems: Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Cognitive Modeling, 171–176. Bamberg: Universitäts-Verlag Bamberg. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.7282/T3MP518G

Muldner, Kasia & Hoiting, Leah & Sanger, Leyna & Blumenfeld, Lev & Toivonen, Ida. 2019. The phonetics of code-switched vowels. International Journal of Bilingualism 23(1). 37–52. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/1367006917709093

Myers-Scotton, Carol. 1997. Duelling languages: Grammatical structure in codeswitching. Oxford University Press.

Myers-Scotton, Carol & Jake, Janice. 2009. A universal model of code-switching and bilingual language processing and production. In Bullock, Barbara & Toribio, Almeida Jacqueline (eds), The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Code-Switching, 336–357. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511576331.020

Ojeda, Adriana & de Prada Pérez, Ana & Wayland, Ratree. 2018. Heritage Speakers and their Language Use: A Phonetic Approach to Code-Switching. In Thompson, Ellen (ed.), Florida Linguistics Papers 5(1).

Olson, Daniel. 2012. The phonetics of insertional code-switching: Suprasegmental analysis and a case for hyper-articulation. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(4). 439–457. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.2.4.05ols

Olson, Daniel. 2013. Bilingual language switching and selection at the phonetic level: Asymmetrical transfer in VOT production. Journal of Phonetics 41(6). 407–420. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.07.005

Olson, Daniel & Ortega-Llebaria, Marta. 2010. The perceptual relevance of code switching and intonation in creating narrow focus. In Selected proceedings of the 4th Conference on Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology, 57–68.

Pfaff, Carol W. 1979. Constraints on language mixing: Intrasentential code-switching and borrowing in Spanish/English. Language 55(2). 291–318. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.2307/412586

Piccinini, Page & Arvaniti, Amalia. 2015. Voice onset time in Spanish-English spontaneous code-switching. Journal of Phonetics 52. 121–137. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.07.004

Poplack, Shana. 1980. Sometimes I’ll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en Español: Toward a typology of code-switching. Linguistics 18(7–8). 581–618. http://hdl.handle.net/10315/2506. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1980.18.7-8.581

Poplack, Shana. 1988. Contrasting patterns of code-switching in two communities. In Heller, Monica (ed.), Codeswitching: Anthropological and Sociolinguistic Perspectives 48. 215–244. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

Poplack, Shana & Meechan, Marjory. 1998. Introduction: How languages fit together in codemixing. International Journal of Bilingualism 2(2). 127–138. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1177/136700699800200201

Poplack, Shana & Robillard, Suzanne & Dion, Nathalie & Paolillo, John C. 2020. Revisiting phonetic integration in bilingual borrowing. Language 96(1). 126–159. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2020.0004

Price, Cathy J. & Green, David W. & Von Studnitz, Roswitha. 1999. A functional imaging study of translation and language switching. Brain 122(12). 2221–2235. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.12.2221

Rogers, Brandon M. A. & Alvord, Scott M. 2019. Laterals in contact: Miami-Cuban Spanish and English /l/. In Valentín-Márquez, Wilfredo & González-Rivera, Melvin (eds.), Dialects from Tropical Islands: Caribbean Spanish in the United States, 54–70. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.4324/9781315115443-4

Sabourin, Laura & Brien, Christie & Burkholder, Michèle. 2014. The effect of age of L2 acquisition on the organization of the bilingual lexicon: Evidence from masked priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 17(3). 542–555. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000643

Santa Ana, Otto & Bayley, Robert. 2008. Chicano English: Phonology. In Kortmann, Bernd & Schneider, Edgar W. (eds.), The Americas and the Caribbean. 219–238. Berlin, New York: De Gruyter Mouton. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1515/9783110208405.1.219

Schepens, Job & Dijkstra, Ton & Grootjen, Franc. 2012. Distributions of cognates in Europe as based on Levenshtein distance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15(1). 157–166. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000623

Shademan, Shabnam. 2006. Is phonotactic knowledge grammatical knowledge. In Baumer, Donald & Montero, David & Scanlon, Michael (eds.), Proceedings of the 25th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, 371–379. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

Smolensky, Paul & Prince, Alan. 1993. Optimality Theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. In McCarthy, John J. (ed.), Optimality Theory in Phonology, 1–71. Blackwell Publishing. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756171.ch1

Stefanich, Sara & Amaro, Jennifer Cabrelli. 2018. Phonological factors of Spanish/English word-internal code-switching. In Lopéz, Luis (ed.), Code-Switching: Theoretical Questions, Experimental Answers, 195–222. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.19.08ste

Stefanich, Sara & Cabrelli, Jennifer & Hilderman, Dustin & Archibald, John. 2019. The Morphophonology of intraword codeswitching: Representation and processing. Frontiers in Communication 4. 54. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2019.00054

Valdés Kroff, Jorge R. & Dussias, Paola E. & Gerfen, Chip & Perrotti, Lauren & Bajo, M. Teresa. 2017. Experience with code-switching modulates the use of grammatical gender during sentence processing. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 7(2). 163–198. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.15010.val

van Hell, Janet G. & Fernandez, Carla B. & Kootstra, Gerrit Jan & Litcofsky, Kaitlyn A. & Ting, Caitlin Y. 2018. Electrophysiological and experimental-behavioral approaches to the study of intra-sentential code-switching. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 8(1). 134–161. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/lab.16010.van

Vergara, Daniel & López, Luis. 2017. Obliteration after vocabulary insertion. In Bellamy, Kate & Child, Michael W. & González, Paz & Muntendam, Antje & Couto, M. Carmen Parafita (eds.), Multidisciplinary Approaches to Bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone World, 261–282. DOI:  http://doi.org/10.1075/ihll.13.11ver

Villanueva-Reyes, Albert. 2013. Spanish/English Contrastive Phonology. Presentation at ASHA convention in Chicago, Illinois. Retrieved online at https://www.asha.org/Events/convention/handouts/2013/1060-Villanueva-Reyes/ on 11/10/2019.